Psychology is the
science of
behavior and
mental processes. Its immediate goal is to understand individuals and groups by both establishing general principles and researching specific cases. For many, the ultimate goal of psychology is to benefit society. In this field, a professional
practitioner or researcher is called a
psychologist, and can be classified as a
social scientist,
behavioral scientist, or
cognitive scientist. Psychologists attempt to understand the role of
mental functions in individual and
social behavior, while also exploring the
physiological and
neurobiological processes that underlie certain functions and behaviors.
Psychologists explore such concepts as
perception,
cognition,
attention,
emotion,
phenomenology,
motivation,
brain functioning,
personality,
behavior, and
interpersonal relationships. Some, especially
depth psychologists, also consider the
unconscious mind. Psychologists employ
empirical methods to infer
causal and
correlational relationships between psychosocial
variables. In
addition, or in
opposition, to employing
empirical and
deductive methods, some—especially
clinical and
counseling psychologists—at times rely upon
symbolic interpretation and other
inductive techniques. Psychology incorporates research from the
social sciences,
natural sciences, and
humanities, such as
philosophy.
While psychological knowledge is often applied to the assessment and treatment of mental health problems, it is also applied to understanding and solving problems in many different spheres of human activity. Although the majority of psychologists are involved in some kind of therapeutic role (clinical, counseling, and school positions), many do scientific research on a wide range of topics related to mental processes and behavior (typically in university psychology departments) or teach such knowledge in academic settings; and some are employed in industrial and organizational settings, and in other areas such as human development and aging, sports, health, the media, law, and forensics.
Etymology
The word ''psychology'' literally means, according to "Webster's Dictionary", "the study of the
soul" (
ψυχή, ''psukhē'', meaning "breath", "spirit", or "soul"; and -λογία
''-logia'', translated as "study of" or "research"). The Latin word ''psychologia'' was first used by the
Croatian
humanist and
Latinist Marko Marulić in his book, ''Psichiologia de ratione animae humanae'' in the late 15th century or early 16th century. The earliest known reference to the word ''psychology'' in English was by
Steven Blankaart in 1694 in ''The Physical Dictionary'' which refers to "Anatomy, which treats of the Body, and Psychology, which treats of the Soul."
History
The study of psychology in a
philosophical context dates back to the ancient civilizations of
Egypt,
Greece,
China,
India, and
Persia. Historians point to the writings of ancient Greek philosophers, such as
Thales,
Plato, and
Aristotle (especially in his ''
De Anima'' treatise), as the first significant body of work in the West to be rich in psychological thought.
Structuralism
German physician
Wilhelm Wundt is credited with introducing psychological discovery into a
laboratory setting. Known as the "father of
experimental psychology", he founded the first psychological laboratory, at
Leipzig University, in 1879. Wundt focused on breaking down mental processes into the most basic components.
Edward Titchener was another major structuralist thinker.
Functionalism
Functionalism formed as a reaction to the theories of the structuralist school of thought and was heavily influenced by the work of the American philosopher, scientist and psychologist
William James. James felt that psychology should have practical value, and that psychologists should find out how the mind can function to a person's benefit. In his book, ''
Principles of Psychology'', published in 1890, he laid the foundations for many of the questions that psychologists would explore for years to come. Other major functionalist thinkers included
John Dewey and
Harvey Carr.
Other 19th-century contributors to the field include the German psychologist Hermann Ebbinghaus, a pioneer in the experimental study of memory, who developed quantitative models of learning and forgetting at the University of Berlin; and the Russian-Soviet physiologist Ivan Pavlov, who discovered in dogs a learning process that was later termed "classical conditioning" and applied to human beings.
Starting in the 1950s, the experimental techniques set forth by Wundt, James, Ebbinghaus, and others would be reiterated as experimental psychology became increasingly cognitive—concerned with information and its processing—and, eventually, constituted a part of the wider cognitive science. In its early years, this development had been seen as a "revolution", as it both responded to and reacted against strains of thought—including psychodynamics and behaviorism—that had developed in the meantime.
Psychoanalysis
From the 1890s until his death in 1939, the Austrian physician
Sigmund Freud developed
psychoanalysis, a method of investigation of the mind and the way one thinks; a systematized set of theories about human behavior; and a form of
psychotherapy to treat psychological or emotional distress, especially unconscious conflict. Freud's psychoanalytic theory was largely based on interpretive methods,
introspection and clinical observations. It became very well known, largely because it tackled subjects such as
sexuality,
repression, and the
unconscious mind as general aspects of psychological development. These were largely considered
taboo subjects at the time, and Freud provided a catalyst for them to be openly discussed in polite society. Clinically, Freud helped to pioneer the method of
free association and a therapeutic interest in
dream interpretation.
Freud had a significant influence on Swiss psychiatrist Carl Jung, whose analytical psychology became an alternative form of depth psychology. Other well-known psychoanalytic scholars of the mid-20th century included psychoanalysts, psychologists, psychiatrists, and philosophers. Among these thinkers were Erik Erikson, Melanie Klein, D.W. Winnicott, Karen Horney, Erich Fromm, John Bowlby, and Sigmund Freud's daughter, Anna Freud. Throughout the 20th century, psychoanalysis evolved into diverse schools of thought, most of which may be classed as Neo-Freudian.
Psychoanalytic theory and therapy were criticized by psychologists such as Hans Eysenck, and by philosophers including Karl Popper. Popper, a philosopher of science, argued that psychoanalysis had been misrepresented as a scientific discipline, whereas Eysenck said that psychoanalytic tenets had been contradicted by experimental data. By the 20th century, psychology departments in American universities had become scientifically oriented, marginalizing Freudian theory and dismissing it as a "desiccated and dead" historical artifact. Meanwhile, however, researchers in the emerging field of neuro-psychoanalysis defended some of Freud's ideas on scientific grounds, while scholars of the humanities maintained that Freud was not a "scientist at all, but ... an interpreter."
Behaviorism
In the United States, behaviorism became the dominant school of thought during the 1950s. Behaviorism was founded in the early 20th century by
John B. Watson, and embraced and extended by
Edward Thorndike,
Clark L. Hull,
Edward C. Tolman, and later
B.F. Skinner. Theories of learning emphasized the ways in which people might be predisposed, or conditioned, by their environments to behave in certain ways.
Classical conditioning was an early behaviorist model. It posited that behavioral tendencies are determined by immediate associations between various environmental stimuli and the degree of pleasure or pain that follows. Behavioral patterns, then, were understood to consist of organisms' conditioned responses to the stimuli in their environment. The stimuli were held to exert influence in proportion to their prior repetition or to the previous intensity of their associated pain or pleasure. Much research consisted of laboratory-based animal experimentation, which was increasing in popularity as physiology grew more sophisticated.
Skinner's behaviorism shared with its predecessors a philosophical inclination toward positivism and determinism. He believed that the contents of the mind were not open to scientific scrutiny and that scientific psychology should emphasize the study of observable behavior. He focused on behavior–environment relations and analyzed overt and covert (i.e., private) behavior as a function of the organism interacting with its environment. Behaviorists usually rejected or deemphasized dualistic explanations such as "mind" or "consciousness"; and, in lieu of probing an "unconscious mind" that underlies unawareness, they spoke of the "contingency-shaped behaviors" in which unawareness becomes outwardly manifest.
Among the behaviorists' most famous creations are John B. Watson's Little Albert experiment, which applied classical conditioning to the developing human child, and Skinner's notion of operant conditioning, which acknowledged that human agency could affect patterns and cycles of environmental stimuli and behavioral responses.
Linguist Noam Chomsky's critique of the behaviorist model of language acquisition is widely regarded as a key factor in the decline of behaviorism's prominence. Martin Seligman and colleagues discovered that the conditioning of dogs led to outcomes ("learned helplessness") that opposed the predictions of behaviorism. But Skinner's behaviorism did not die, perhaps in part because it generated successful practical applications. The fall of behaviorism as an overarching model in psychology, however, gave way to a new dominant paradigm: cognitive approaches.
Humanism
Humanistic psychology was developed in the 1950s in reaction to both
behaviorism and psychoanalysis. By using
phenomenology,
intersubjectivity and first-person categories, the humanistic approach sought to glimpse the whole person—not just the fragmented parts of the personality or cognitive functioning. Humanism focused on fundamentally and uniquely human issues, such as individual free will, personal growth,
self-actualization,
self-identity,
death,
aloneness,
freedom, and
meaning. The humanistic approach was distinguished by its emphasis on subjective meaning, rejection of determinism, and concern for positive growth rather than pathology. Some of the founders of the humanistic school of thought were American psychologists
Abraham Maslow, who formulated a
hierarchy of human needs, and
Carl Rogers, who created and developed
client-centered therapy. Later,
positive psychology opened up humanistic themes to scientific modes of exploration.
Gestalt
Wolfgang Kohler,
Max Wertheimer and
Kurt Koffka co-founded the school of
Gestalt psychology. This approach is based upon the idea that individuals experience things as unified wholes. This approach to psychology began in Germany and Austria during the late 19th century in response to the molecular approach of structuralism. Rather than breaking down thoughts and behavior to their smallest element, the Gestalt position maintains that the whole of experience is important, and the whole is different than the sum of its parts.
Gestalt psychology should not be confused with the Gestalt therapy of Fritz Perls, which is only peripherally linked to Gestalt psychology.
Existentialism
Influenced largely by the work of
German philosopher
Martin Heidegger and
Danish philosopher
Søren Kierkegaard, psychoanalytically trained
American psychologist
Rollo May pioneered an
existential branch of psychology, which included
existential therapy, in the 1950s and 1960s. Existential psychologists differed from others often classified as humanistic in their comparatively neutral view of
human nature and in their relatively positive assessment of
anxiety. Existential psychologists emphasized the humanistic themes of death, free will, and meaning, suggesting that meaning can be shaped by
myths, or narrative patterns, and that it can be encouraged by an acceptance of the free will requisite to an
authentic, albeit often anxious, regard for death and other future prospects. Austrian existential psychiatrist and
Holocaust survivor
Viktor Frankl drew evidence of meaning's therapeutic power from reflections garnered from his own
internment, and he created a variety of existential psychotherapy called
logotherapy. In addition to May and Frankl, Swiss psychoanalyst
Ludwig Binswanger and American psychologist
George Kelly may be said to belong to the existential school.
Cognitivism
Cognitive psychology is the branch of psychology that studies mental processes including how people think, perceive, remember, and learn. As part of the larger field of cognitive science, this branch of psychology is related to other disciplines including neuroscience, philosophy, and linguistics.
Noam Chomsky helped to ignite a "cognitive revolution" in psychology when he criticized the behaviorists' notions of "stimulus", "response", and "reinforcement", arguing that such ideas—which Skinner had borrowed from animal experiments in the laboratory—could be applied to complex human behavior, most notably language acquisition, in only a vague and superficial manner. The postulation that humans are born with the instinct or "innate facility" for acquiring language posed a challenge to the behaviorist position that all behavior (including language) is contingent upon learning and reinforcement. Social learning theorists, such as Albert Bandura, argued that the child's environment could make contributions of its own to the behaviors of an observant subject.
Meanwhile, accumulating technology helped to renew interest and belief in the mental states and representations—i.e., the cognition—that had fallen out of favor with behaviorists. English neuroscientist Charles Sherrington and Canadian psychologist Donald O. Hebb used experimental methods to link psychological phenomena with the structure and function of the brain. With the rise of computer science and artificial intelligence, analogies were drawn between the processing of information by humans and information processing by machines. Research in cognition had proven practical since World War II, when it aided in the understanding of weapons operation. By the late 20th century, though, cognitivism had become the dominant paradigm of mainstream psychology, and cognitive psychology emerged as a popular branch.
Assuming both that the covert mind should be studied and that the scientific method should be used to study it, cognitive psychologists set such concepts as "subliminal processing" and "implicit memory" in place of the psychoanalytic "unconscious mind" or the behavioristic "contingency-shaped behaviors". Elements of behaviorism and cognitive psychology were synthesized to form the basis of cognitive behavioral therapy, a form of psychotherapy modified from techniques developed by American psychologist Albert Ellis and American psychiatrist Aaron T. Beck. Cognitive psychology was subsumed along with other disciplines, such as philosophy of mind, computer science, and neuroscience, under the umbrella discipline of cognitive science.
Biopsychosocial model
The biopsychosocial model is an integrated perspective toward understanding consciousness, behavior, and social interaction. It assumes that any given behavior or mental process affects and is affected by dynamically interrelated biological, psychological, and social factors. The ''psychological'' aspect refers to the role that cognition and emotions play in any given psychological phenomenon—for example, the effect of mood or beliefs and expectations on an individual's reactions to an event. The ''biological'' aspect refers to the role of biological factors in psychological phenomena—for example, the effect of the prenatal environment on brain development and cognitive abilities, or the influence of genes on individual dispositions. The ''socio-cultural'' aspect refers to the role that social and cultural environments play in a given psychological phenomenon—for example, the role of parental or peer influence in the behaviors or characteristics of an individual.
Discursive psychology
Discursive psychology was developed in the 1990s by Jonathan Potter and Derek Edwards. It examines how psychological phenomena are created, made relevant and put to use in discourse, verbal interaction and everyday talk. It is opposed to cognitivist approaches.
Critical psychology
Critical psychology is aimed at critiquing mainstream psychology and attempts to apply psychology in more progressive ways, often looking towards social change as a means of preventing and treating psychopathology. One of critical psychology's main criticisms of conventional psychology is how it ignores the way power differences between social classes and groups can impact the mental and physical well-being of individuals or groups of people. Contributors to the field include
Klaus Holzkamp and
Ian Parker. Key elements within critical psychology include the study of power relations, situated knowledge, and the dualism of the self and agency and the dualism of the individual and the social.
Subfields
Psychology encompasses a vast domain, and includes many different approaches to the study of mental processes and behavior.
Biological
Biological psychology or
behavioral neuroscience is the study of the biological substrates of behavior and mental processes. There are different specialties within behavioral neuroscience. For example,
physiological psychologists use animal models (typically rats) to study the neural, genetic, and cellular mechanisms that underlie specific behaviors such as learning and memory and fear responses.
Cognitive neuroscientists investigate the neural correlates of psychological processes in humans using neural imaging tools, and
neuropsychologists conduct psychological assessments to determine, for instance, specific aspects and extent of cognitive deficit caused by brain damage or disease.
Clinical
Clinical psychology includes the study and application of psychology for the purpose of understanding, preventing, and relieving psychologically-based distress or
dysfunction and to promote subjective
well-being and personal development. Central to its practice are psychological assessment and
psychotherapy, although clinical psychologists may also engage in research, teaching, consultation, forensic testimony, and program development and administration. Some clinical psychologists may focus on the clinical management of patients with
brain injury—this area is known as
clinical neuropsychology. In many countries, clinical psychology is a regulated
mental health profession.
The work performed by clinical psychologists tends to be influenced by various therapeutic approaches, all of which involve a formal relationship between professional and client (usually an individual, couple, family, or small group). The various therapeutic approaches and practices are associated with different theoretical perspectives and employ different procedures intended to form a therapeutic alliance, explore the nature of psychological problems, and encourage new ways of thinking, feeling, or behaving. Four major theoretical perspectives are psychodynamic, cognitive behavioral, existential–humanistic, and systems or family therapy. There has been a growing movement to integrate the various therapeutic approaches, especially with an increased understanding of issues regarding culture, gender, spirituality, and sexual-orientation. With the advent of more robust research findings regarding psychotherapy, there is evidence that most of the major therapies are about of equal effectiveness, with the key common element being a strong therapeutic alliance. Because of this, more training programs and psychologists are now adopting an eclectic therapeutic orientation.
Cognitive
Cognitive psychology studies
cognition, the
mental processes underlying mental activity.
Perception,
learning,
problem solving,
reasoning,
thinking,
memory,
attention,
language and
emotion are areas of research. Classical cognitive psychology is associated with a school of thought known as
cognitivism, whose adherents argue for an
information processing model of mental function, informed by
functionalism and
experimental psychology.
On a broader level, cognitive science is an interdisciplinary enterprise of cognitive psychologists, cognitive neuroscientists, researchers in artificial intelligence, linguists, human–computer interaction, computational neuroscience, logicians and social scientists. Computational models are sometimes used to simulate phenomena of interest. Computational models provide a tool for studying the functional organization of the mind whereas neuroscience provides measures of brain activity.
Comparative
Comparative psychology refers to the study of the behavior and mental life of
animals other than human beings. It is related to disciplines outside of psychology that study animal behavior such as
ethology. Although the field of psychology is primarily concerned with humans, the behavior and mental processes of
animals is also an important part of psychological research. This being either as a subject in its own right (e.g.,
animal cognition and ethology) or with strong emphasis about evolutionary links, and somewhat more controversially, as a way of gaining an insight into human psychology. This is achieved by means of comparison or via animal models of emotional and behavior systems as seen in neuroscience of psychology (e.g.,
affective neuroscience and
social neuroscience).
Developmental
Mainly focusing on the development of the human mind through the life span,
developmental psychology seeks to understand how people come to perceive, understand, and act within the world and how these processes change as they age. This may focus on intellectual, cognitive, neural, social, or
moral development. Researchers who study children use a number of unique research methods to make observations in natural settings or to engage them in experimental tasks. Such tasks often resemble specially designed games and activities that are both enjoyable for the child and scientifically useful, and researchers have even devised clever methods to study the mental processes of small infants. In addition to studying children, developmental psychologists also study
aging and processes throughout the life span, especially at other times of rapid change (such as adolescence and old age). Developmental psychologists draw on the full range of psychological theories to inform their research.
Educational and school
Educational psychology is the study of how humans learn in
educational settings, the effectiveness of educational interventions, the psychology of teaching, and the
social psychology of
schools as organizations. The work of child psychologists such as
Lev Vygotsky,
Jean Piaget,
Bernard Luskin and
Jerome Bruner has been influential in creating
teaching methods and educational practices. Educational psychology is often included in teacher education programs, in places such as North America, Australia, and New Zealand.
School psychology combines principles from educational psychology and clinical psychology to understand and treat students with learning disabilities; to foster the intellectual growth of gifted students; to facilitate prosocial behaviors in adolescents; and otherwise to promote safe, supportive, and effective learning environments. School psychologists are trained in educational and behavioral assessment, intervention, prevention, and consultation, and many have extensive training in research.
Evolutionary
Evolutionary psychology examines psychological
traits—such as
memory,
perception, or
language—from a
modern evolutionary perspective. It seeks to identify which human psychological traits are evolved
adaptations, that is, the functional products of
natural selection or
sexual selection. Evolutionary psychologists suggest that
psychological adaptations evolved to solve recurrent problems in human ancestral environments. By focusing on the evolution of psychological traits and their adaptive functions, it offers complementary explanations for the mostly proximate or developmental explanations developed by other areas of psychology (that is, it focuses mostly on ultimate or "why?" questions, rather than proximate or "how?" questions).
Industrial–organizational
Industrial and organizational psychology (I–O) applies psychological concepts and methods to optimize human potential in the workplace. Personnel psychology, a subfield of I–O psychology, applies the methods and principles of psychology in selecting and evaluating workers. I–O psychology's other subfield,
organizational psychology, examines the effects of work environments and management styles on worker motivation, job satisfaction, and productivity.
Personality
Personality psychology is concerned to enduring patterns of
behavior,
thought, and
emotion in individuals, commonly referred to as
personality. Theories of personality vary across different psychological schools and orientations. They carry different assumptions about such issues as the role of the
unconscious and the importance of childhood experience. According to Freud, personality is based on the dynamic interactions of the
id, ego, and super-ego.
Trait theorists, in contrast, attempt to analyze personality in terms of a discrete number of key traits by the statistical method of
factor analysis. The number of proposed traits has varied widely. An early model proposed by
Hans Eysenck suggested that there are three traits that comprise human personality:
extraversion–introversion,
neuroticism, and
psychoticism.
Raymond Cattell proposed a theory of
16 personality factors. The "
Big Five", or Five Factor Model, proposed by
Lewis Goldberg, currently has strong support among trait theorists.
Social
Social psychology is the study of how humans think about each other and how they relate to each other. Social psychologists study such topics as the influence of others on an individual's behavior (e.g.
conformity,
persuasion), and the formation of beliefs,
attitudes, and
stereotypes about other people.
Social cognition fuses elements of social and cognitive psychology in order to understand how people process, remember, and distort social information. The study of
group dynamics reveals information about the nature and potential optimization of leadership, communication, and other phenomena that emerge at least at the
microsocial level. In recent years, many social psychologists have become increasingly interested in
implicit measures,
mediational models, and the interaction of both person and social variables in accounting for behavior. The study of human society, is therefore a potentially valuable source of information about the causes of psychiatric disorder. Some of the sociological concepts applied to psychiatric disorders are the social role, sick role, social class, life event, culture, migration, social and total institution.
Positive
Positive psychology derives from Maslow's humanistic psychology. Positive psychology is a discipline that utilizes evidence-based scientific methods to study factors that contribute to human happiness and strength. Different from clinical psychology, positive psychology is concerned with improving the mental well-being of healthy clients. Positive psychological interventions now have received tentative support for their beneficial effects on clients. In 2010 ''Clinical Psychological Review'' published a special issue devoted to positive psychological interventions, such as gratitude journaling and the physical expression of gratitude. There is, however, a need for further research on the effects of interventions. Positive psychological interventions have been limited in scope but their effects are thought to be superior to that of placebos, especially with regard to helping people with body image problems.
Research methods
Psychology tends to be
eclectic, drawing on knowledge from other fields to help explain and understand psychological phenomena. Additionally, psychologists make extensive use of
the three modes of inference that were identified by
C.S. Peirce:
deduction,
induction, and
abduction (hypothesis generation). While often employing
deductive–nomological reasoning, they also rely on
inductive reasoning to generate
explanations. For example,
evolutionary psychologists attempt to explain psychological traits—such as memory, perception, or language—as adaptations, that is, as the functional products of
natural selection or
sexual selection.
Psychologists may conduct basic research aiming for further understanding in a particular area of interest in psychology, or conduct applied research to solve problems in the clinic, workplace or other areas. Masters level clinical programs aim to train students in both research methods and evidence-based practice. Professional associations have established guidelines for ethics, training, research methodology and professional practice. In addition, depending on the country, state or region, psychological services and the title "psychologist" may be governed by statute and psychologists who offer services to the public are usually required to be licensed.
Qualitative and quantitative research
Research in most areas of psychology is conducted in accord with the standards of the
scientific method. Psychological researchers seek the emergence of theoretically interesting categories and hypotheses from data, using
qualitative or
quantitative methods (or both).
Qualitative psychological research methods include interviews, first-hand observation, and participant observation. Creswell (2003) identifies five main possibilities for quantitative research, including narrative, phenomenology, ethnography, case study and grounded theory. Qualitative researchers sometimes aim to enrich interpretations or critiques of symbols, subjective experiences, or social structures. Similar hermeneutic and critical aims have also been served by "quantitative methods", as in Erich Fromm's study of Nazi voting or Stanley Milgram's studies of obedience to authority.
Quantitative psychological research lends itself to the statistical testing of hypotheses. Quantitatively oriented research designs include the experiment, quasi-experiment, cross-sectional study, case-control study, and longitudinal study. The measurement and operationalization of important constructs is an essential part of these research designs. Statistical methods include the Pearson product–moment correlation coefficient, the analysis of variance, multiple linear regression, logistic regression, structural equation modeling, and hierarchical linear modeling.
Controlled experiments
Experimental psychological research is conducted in a laboratory under controlled conditions. This method of research relies on the application of the
scientific method to understand behavior. Experimenters use several types of measurements, including rate of response,
reaction time, and various
psychometric measurements. Experiments are designed to
test specific
hypotheses (deductive approach) or evaluate functional relationships (inductive approach). A
true experiment with
random allocation of subjects to conditions allows researchers to infer causal relationships between different aspects of behavior and the environment. In an experiment, one or more variables of interest are controlled by the experimenter (independent variable) and another variable is measured in response to different conditions (dependent variable). Experiments are one of the primary research methods in many areas of psychology, particularly
cognitive/
psychonomics,
mathematical psychology,
psychophysiology and
biological psychology/
cognitive neuroscience.
Experiments on humans have been put under some controls, namely informed and voluntary consent. After World War II, the Nuremberg Code was established, because of Nazi abuses of experimental subjects. Later, most countries (and scientific journals) adopted the Declaration of Helsinki. In the U.S., the National Institutes of Health established the Institutional Review Board in 1966, and in 1974 adopted the National Research Act (HR 7724). All of these measures encouraged researchers to obtain informed consent from human participants in experimental studies. A number of influential studies led to the establishment of this rule; such studies included the MIT and Fernald School radioisotope studies, the Thalidomide tragedy, the Willowbrook hepatitis study, and Stanley Milgram's studies of obedience to authority.
Survey questionnaires
Statistical surveys are used in psychology for measuring attitudes and traits, monitoring changes in mood, checking the validity of experimental manipulations, and for a wide variety of other psychological topics. Most commonly, psychologists use paper-and-pencil surveys. However, surveys are also conducted over the phone or through e-mail. Increasingly, web-based surveys are being used in research. Similar methodology is also used in applied setting, such as clinical assessment and personnel assessment.
Longitudinal studies
Longitudinal studies are often used in psychology to study developmental trends across the life span, and in sociology to study life events throughout lifetimes or generations. The reason for this is that unlike
cross-sectional studies, longitudinal studies track the same people, and therefore the differences observed in those people are less likely to be the result of cultural differences across generations. Because of this benefit, longitudinal studies make observing changes more accurate and they are applied in various other fields.
Because most longitudinal studies are observational, in the sense that they observe the state of the world without manipulating it, it has been argued that they may have less power to detect causal relationships than do experiments. They also suffer methodological limitations such as from selective attrition because people with similar characteristics may be more likely to drop out of the study making it difficult to analyze.
Some longitudinal studies are experiments, called repeated-measures experiments. Psychologists often use the crossover design to reduce the influence of confounding covariates and to reduce the number of subjects.
Observation in natural settings
In the same way
Jane Goodall studied the role of
chimpanzee social and family life, psychologists conduct similar observational studies in human social, professional and family lives. Sometimes the participants are aware they are being observed and other times it is covert: the participants do not know they are being observed. Ethical guidelines need to be taken into consideration when covert observation is being carried out.
Qualitative and descriptive research
Research designed to answer questions about the current state of affairs such as the thoughts, feelings and behaviors of individuals is known as descriptive research. Descriptive research can be qualitative or quantitative in orientation. Qualitative research is descriptive research that is focused on observing and describing events as they occur, with the goal of capturing all of the richness of everyday behavior and with the hope of discovering and understanding phenomena that might have been missed if only more cursory examinations have been made.
Neuropsychological methods
Neuropsychology seeks to connect aspects of behavior and mental activity with the structure and function of the
brain.
Cognitive neuropsychology and
cognitive neuropsychiatry study neurological or mental impairment in an attempt to infer theories of normal mind and brain function. This typically involves looking for differences in patterns of remaining ability (known as "functional disassociations") which can give clues as to whether abilities are composed of smaller functions, or are controlled by a single cognitive mechanism.
In addition, experimental techniques are often used to study the neuropsychology of healthy individuals. These include behavioral experiments, brain-scanning or functional neuroimaging, used to examine the activity of the brain during task performance, and techniques such as transcranial magnetic stimulation, which can safely alter the function of small brain areas to reveal their importance in mental operations.
Computational modeling
Computational modeling is a tool often used in
mathematical psychology and
cognitive psychology to simulate a particular behavior using a computer. This method has several advantages. Since modern computers process extremely quickly, many simulations can be run in a short time, allowing for a great deal of statistical power. Modeling also allows psychologists to visualize hypotheses about the functional organization of mental events that couldn't be directly observed in a human.
Several different types of modeling are used to study behavior. Connectionism uses neural networks to simulate the brain. Another method is symbolic modeling, which represents many different mental objects using variables and rules. Other types of modeling include dynamic systems and stochastic modeling.
Animal studies
Animal learning experiments aid in investigating the biological basis of teaching, memory and behavior. In the 1890s, Russian physiologist
Ivan Pavlov famously used dogs to demonstrate
classical conditioning.
Non-human primates, cats, dogs, pigeons,
rats and other
rodents are often used in psychological experiments. Ideally, controlled experiments introduce only one
independent variable at a time, in order to ascertain its unique effects upon dependent variables. These conditions are approximated best in laboratory settings. In contrast, human environments and genetic backgrounds vary so widely, and depend upon so many factors, that it is difficult to control important variables for human subjects. Of course, there are pitfalls in generalizing findings from animal studies to humans although animal models can be helpful in developing an understanding of human behavior (e.g., addiction research).
Criticism
Theory
Criticisms of psychological research often come from perceptions that it is a "soft" science. Philosopher of science
Thomas Kuhn's 1962 critique implied psychology overall was in a pre-paradigm state, lacking the agreement on overarching theory found in mature sciences such as
chemistry and
physics.
Because some areas of psychology rely on research methods such as surveys and questionnaires, critics have asserted that psychology is not an objective science. Other phenomena that psychologists are interested in, such as personality, thinking, and emotion, cannot be directly measured and are often inferred from subjective self-reports, which may be problematic.
Misuses of hypothesis-testing in psychology, and the use of hypothesis testing at all is controversial. Research has documented that many psychologists confuse statistical significance with practical importance. Statistically significant but practically unimportant results are common with large samples. Some psychologists have responded with an increased use of effect size statistics, rather than sole reliance on the Fisherian ''p'' < .05 significance criterion (whereby an observed difference is deemed "statistically significant" if an effect of that size or larger would occur with 5% (or less) probability in independent replications, assuming the truth of the null-hypothesis of no difference between the treatments).
Sometimes the debate comes from within psychology, for example between laboratory-oriented researchers and practitioners such as clinicians. In recent years, and particularly in the U.S., there has been increasing debate about the nature of therapeutic effectiveness and about the relevance of empirically examining psychotherapeutic strategies.
Practice
Some observers perceive a gap between scientific theory and its application—in particular, the application of unsupported or unsound clinical practices. Critics say there has been an increase in the number of mental health training programs that do not instill scientific competence. One skeptic asserts that practices, such as "
facilitated communication for infantile autism"; memory-recovery techniques including
body work; and other therapies, such as
rebirthing and
reparenting, may be dubious or even dangerous, despite their popularity. In 1984, Allen Neuringer had made a similar point regarding the experimental analysis of behavior.
Current ethical standards of psychology would not permit the following studies to be conducted today. These human studies would violate the Ethics Code of the American Psychological Association, the Canadian Code of Conduct for Research Involving Humans, and the Belmont Report. Current ethical guidelines state that using non-human animals for scientific purposes is only acceptable when the harm (physical or psychological) done to animals is outweighed by the benefits of the research. Keeping this in mind, psychologists can use on animals research techniques that would not necessarily be performed on humans.
An experiment by Stanley Milgram raised questions about the ethics of scientific experimentation because of the extreme emotional stress suffered by the participants. It measured the willingness of study participants to obey an authority figure who instructed them to perform acts that conflicted with their personal conscience.
Harry Harlow drew condemnation for his "pit of despair" experiments on rhesus macaque monkeys at the University of Wisconsin–Madison in the 1970s. The aim of the research was to produce an animal model of clinical depression. Harlow also devised what he called a "rape rack", to which the female isolates were tied in normal monkey mating posture. In 1974, American literary critic Wayne C. Booth wrote that, "Harry Harlow and his colleagues go on torturing their nonhuman primates decade after decade, invariably proving what we all knew in advance—that social creatures can be destroyed by destroying their social ties." He writes that Harlow made no mention of the criticism of the morality of his work.
University psychology departments have ethics committees dedicated to the rights and well-being of research subjects. Researchers in psychology must gain approval of their research projects before conducting any experiment to protect the interests of human participants and laboratory animals.
Systemic bias
In 1959 statistician Theodore Sterling examined the results of psychological studies and discovered that 97% of them supported their initial hypotheses, implying a possible
publication bias. Similarly Fanelli (2010) found out that 91.5% of psychiatry/psychology studies confirmed the effects they were looking for, which was around five times more often than in
space- or
geosciences. Fanelli argues that this is because of researchers in "softer" sciences have fewer constraints to their conscious and unconscious biases.
In November 2010, ''New Scientist'' Magazine reported a systemic bias in psychology studies towards WEIRD ("western, educated, industrialized, rich and democratic") subjects. Although only 1/8 people worldwide fall into the WEIRD classification, ''New Scientist'' claimed that 60–90% of psychology studies are performed on WEIRD subjects. The article gave examples of results that differ significantly between WEIRD subjects and tribal cultures, including the Müller-Lyer illusion.
See also
Artificial intelligence
Consciousness
Family therapy
Group therapy
List of psychologists
List of psychology organizations
List of publications in psychology
Philosophy of mind
Philosophy of psychology
Recept
Sociology
Social Work
Notes
Although
psychoanalysis and other forms of depth psychology are most typically associated with the unconscious mind,
behaviorists consider such phenomena as
classical conditioning and
operant conditioning, while
cognitivists explore
implicit memory,
automaticity, and
subliminal messages, all of which are understood either to bypass or to occur outside of conscious effort or attention. Indeed,
cognitive-behavioral therapists counsel their clients to become aware of maladaptive thought patterns, the nature of which the clients previously had not been conscious.
Among these schools are
ego psychology,
object relations, and
interpersonal,
Lacanian, and
relational psychoanalysis. Modification of Jung's theories led to the
archetypal and
process-oriented schools.
For example, scientists have related brain structures to Freudian concepts such as
libido,
drives, the
unconscious, and
repression. The contributors to neuro-psychoanalysis include
António Damásio,
Eric Kandel,
Joseph E. LeDoux,
Jaak Panksepp,
Oliver Sacks,
Mark Solms, and
Douglas Watt.
Gregg Henriques of
James Madison University, for example, published his
Tree of Knowledge System in 2003 as a proposal for the theoretical unification of psychology. For a general discussion and critique, see also
Mark Jarzombek's book, ''The Psychologizing of Modernity''.
References
External links
American Psychological Association
Association for Psychological Science
TeachPsychScience
Category:Behavioural sciences
Category:Greek loanwords
Category:Neuroscience
af:Sielkunde
ak:Saekɔlɔgyi
ar:علم النفس
an:Psicolochía
ast:Sicoloxía
az:Psixologiya
bn:মনোবিজ্ঞান
zh-min-nan:Sim-lí-ha̍k
be:Псіхалогія
be-x-old:Псыхалёгія
bo:སེམས་ཁམས་རིག་པ།
bs:Psihologija
br:Bredoniezh
bg:Психология
ca:Psicologia
cv:Психологи
ceb:Sikolohiya
cs:Psychologie
co:Psicologia
cy:Seicoleg
da:Psykologi
de:Psychologie
et:Psühholoogia
el:Ψυχολογία
es:Psicología
eo:Psikologio
ext:Sicologia
eu:Psikologia
fa:روانشناسی
fo:Sálarfrøði
fr:Psychologie
fy:Psychology
fur:Psicologjie
ga:Síceolaíocht
gv:Shicklaage
gd:Eòlas-inntinn
gl:Psicoloxía
gu:મનોવિજ્ઞાન
ko:심리학
hy:Հոգեբանություն
hi:मानस शास्त्र
hr:Psihologija
io:Psikologio
id:Psikologi
ia:Psychologia
ie:Psichologie
os:Психологи
is:Sálfræði
it:Psicologia
he:פסיכולוגיה
jv:Psikologi
kl:Tarnip ilinniarnera
kn:ಮನೋಶಾಸ್ತ್ರ
ka:ფსიქოლოგია
csb:Psychòlogijô
kk:Психология
ky:Психология
sw:Elimunafsia
ku:Psîkolojî
lad:Psikolojiya
lo:ຈິດຕະວິທະຍາ
la:Psychologia
lv:Psiholoģija
lb:Psychologie
lt:Psichologija
lij:Psicologia
li:Psychologie
jbo:menske
hu:Pszichológia
mk:Психологија
mg:Toe-tsaina
ml:മനഃശാസ്ത്രം
mt:Psikoloġija
mr:मानसशास्त्र
arz:سايكولوجيا
mzn:روانشناسی
ms:Psikologi
mwl:Psicologie
mn:Сэтгэл судлал
my:စိတ်ပညာ
nl:Psychologie
nds-nl:Psychologie
ne:मनोविज्ञान
ja:心理学
nap:Psicologgia
frr:Psükologii
no:Psykologi
nn:Psykologi
nrm:Psychologie
nov:Psikologia
oc:Psicologia
pi:चिन्तनशास्त्र
pnb:نفسیات
ps:ساپوهنه
pcd:Psikolodjie
tpi:Saikolosi
pl:Psychologia
pt:Psicologia
ro:Psihologie
qu:Nuna yachay
rue:Псіхолоґія
ru:Психология
sah:Психология
sm:Saikolosi
sc:Psicologia
sco:Psychology
stq:Psychologie
st:Saekholoji
sq:Psikologjia
scn:Psicoluggìa
si:මනෝවේදය
simple:Psychology
sk:Psychológia
sl:Psihologija
so:Cilminafsi
sr:Психологија
sh:Psihologija
su:Psikologi
fi:Psykologia
sv:Psykologi
tl:Sikolohiya
ta:உளவியல்
tt:Психология
te:మానసిక శాస్త్రము
th:จิตวิทยา
tg:Равоншиносӣ
tr:Psikoloji
uk:Психологія
ur:نفسیات
vec:Sicołogia
vi:Tâm lý học
fiu-vro:Psükoloogia
vls:Psychologie
war:Psikolohiya
yi:פסיכאלאגיע
zh-yue:心理學
diq:Psikolociye
zea:Psyholohie
bat-smg:Psėchologėjė
zh:心理学