Introduction
Mutualism, the cooperative interaction between species, is
ubiquitous and fundamental to life on earth (Bronstein 1994). Mutualistic symbioses are
vital for a range of crucial mechanisms ensuring reproduction and nutrient
acquisition, as well being critical to broader environmental functions such as
the nitrogen cycle and carbon sequestration (Wilson et al. 2009). One of the most important
mutualisms is the relationship between angiosperms and their biotic
pollinators. As with all symbioses, these are in a state of flux over
evolutionary time, with the balance between cooperation and exploitation
changing according to environmental variants and arms races between the
participants, as natural selection pressures them to maximise their own
reproductive success.
While there is a growing and substantial body of research
examining the natural history of mutualism and increasing empirical knowledge
of costs and benefits to symbionts, evolutionary models are limited and
frequently in conflict (Bronstein 2001; Bshary
& Bronstein 2004).
In this review, I will discuss the history of biological
exploration of interspecies cooperation and consider the diversity of mutualism
among flowering plants and their biotic pollinators and the exploitation that
can lead to its destabilisation. In addition, I will survey mechanisms thought
to contribute to the maintenance of mutualistic interactions, and finally,
examine some of the models of mutualism that have been posited.
The evolution of
cooperation: a history
Since Darwin’s initial formulation of evolutionary theory (Darwin 1859), cooperation within and
between species has presented a challenge to the idea of natural selection
favouring individuals which maximise their own fitness through “selfish”
behaviour. An explanation of intraspecific cooperation and altruism emerged
with Hamilton’s proposal of kin selection (Hamilton 1964), whereby it is in the
interest of an individual to foster the well-being of close kin, as these share
a high proportion of genes with that individual. This concept was extended and
popularised by Dawkins, who emphasised the importance of the gene as the unit
of selection, contextualising it in the idea of inclusive fitness (Dawkins 2006).
These rationales for cooperation do not pertain to
interspecific mutualism, however, since the genetic relatedness of different
species that interact to benefit each other can only be comparatively small. Clearly,
kin selection and inclusive fitness do not provide explanations for diverse
species providing each other with services and food. Other explanations needed
to be sought.
Although Darwin was well aware of interspecific mutuality,
and expounded upon examples such as the dispersal of seeds by the mistletoe
bird (Darwin 1859), his essential focus was upon
explaining evolutionary change by the effects of conflict and competition (Sapp 1994). “Darwin’s bulldog”, Thomas
Huxley, emphasised the idea of the Hobbesian war occurring among both humans
and other animals (Huxley 1888), as did the staunch laissez-faire
liberal Herbert Spencer who coined the term, “survival of the fittest” (Spencer 1904).
The Russian Peter Kropotkin recoiled at the idea of competition
and conflict being the essence of evolution as proposed by Darwin and Huxley,
and perhaps influenced by his political ideology of anarchist communism and his
own natural history experiences in eastern Siberia (Sapp 1994), argued that cooperation and
altruism are fundamental to both human nature and species in general (Kropotkin 1902). While Kropotkin himself made
little reference to interspecific mutualism, his ideas profoundly influenced
subsequent zoologists researching this field who found the perceived social
implications of Darwinism repugnant (Sapp 1994). It is perhaps not
coincidental that the originator of the principle of symbiogenesis, Boris
Mikhaylovich Kozo-Polyansky (Kozo-Polyansky 1924), was a countryman of
Kropotkin and educated in Soviet Russia.
In modern times, explanations for biological cooperation are
less infused by political ideology, and are based in evolutionary ecology and
mathematical models of cost and benefit to the participants. Nevertheless,
while the mechanisms of interspecies competition for resources have been
studied in depth, those regulating cooperative behaviour between species have
received far less attention (Bronstein 1994; Stadler
& Dixon 2008)
and the dynamics of mutualistic interaction are subject to continuing debate (Bronstein 2001; Bshary
& Bronstein 2004; Hoeksema & Bruna 2000; Weyl et al. 2010).
The pervasiveness of mutualism
It has been estimated that every species on earth is
involved in at least one cooperative symbiotic relationship with another
species (Bronstein 2001). These mutualisms are found
between all kingdoms of life and exist in every ecosystem thus explored (Paracer & Ahmadjian
2000).
Mutualisms made possible the migration of plants onto land (Pirozynski &
Malloch 1975)
and are considered to have been a crucial element in the evolution of
eukaryotic cells (Margulis 1967). The ubiquity of mutualisms
and symbiosis generally has been described as presenting a unifying concept in
biological sciences (Paracer & Ahmadjian
2000),
without which life as it has evolved on earth would be impossible.
The cooperative association between angiosperms and their
biotic pollinators is ancient and fundamental to the diversity of flowering
plants. Biotic pollination is the transfer by an organism of a male gamete (in
pollen) from the anther of a flower to the stigma of another flower, either of
the same plant (or indeed flower) or a separate individual. Abiotic pollination,
involving transportation of pollen by wind, rain or gravity is also employed by
many angiosperms, and is the primary mechanism for gamete transfer in
gymnosperms such as cycads and conifers.
The success of angiosperms in colonising terrestrial ecosystems throughout the planet is due in large measure to the relative precision of biotic pollination, as it requires the production of fewer gametes for a greater reward. Pollen is high in proteins and sugars, and has been an insect food source since before the evolution of angiosperms, as evidenced by gymnosperm pollen found preserved in insect guts from the Permian (Labandeira 1997). This suggests that the evolution of insect pollination occurred as an exploitation of this behaviour (Bronstein, Alarcón & Geber 2006). Angiosperms and their pollinating insects radiated in parallel during the Cretaceous (Grimaldi & Engel 2005). Traits of both plants and their pollinators are subject to selection by this interaction, resulting in increasing diversity mainly in response to multispecies interactions. Most angiosperms are pollinated by multiple pollinators, and most pollinators visit multiple angiosperms (Fenster et al. 2004).
The success of angiosperms in colonising terrestrial ecosystems throughout the planet is due in large measure to the relative precision of biotic pollination, as it requires the production of fewer gametes for a greater reward. Pollen is high in proteins and sugars, and has been an insect food source since before the evolution of angiosperms, as evidenced by gymnosperm pollen found preserved in insect guts from the Permian (Labandeira 1997). This suggests that the evolution of insect pollination occurred as an exploitation of this behaviour (Bronstein, Alarcón & Geber 2006). Angiosperms and their pollinating insects radiated in parallel during the Cretaceous (Grimaldi & Engel 2005). Traits of both plants and their pollinators are subject to selection by this interaction, resulting in increasing diversity mainly in response to multispecies interactions. Most angiosperms are pollinated by multiple pollinators, and most pollinators visit multiple angiosperms (Fenster et al. 2004).
The evolution of nectar as a means of attracting pollinators
occurred prior to the Late Jurassic (Labandeira 1997). A large proportion of
Australian angiosperms are pollinated by birds, in contrast with plants in
other continents, with plants having evolved specialised morphological
adaptations that facilitate bird feeding and pollination (Ford, Paton & Forde
1979).
Many of these species are also pollinated by mammals. That Australia should be
a hotspot for bird pollination has raised a number of hypotheses. It has been
suggested that the high abundance of sunlight ensures that photosynthesis and
the production of sugars is not a limiting factor in the physiology of many
Australian plants, making nectar a relatively cheap product to exchange for
pollination. Soil nutrients, by contrast, are particularly limiting in areas of
Australia where bird pollination is most common (Ford, Paton & Forde
1979).
This is significantly the case with phosphorus, essential for the synthesis of
proteins (Atwell, Kriedemann
& Turnbull 1999).
Producing excess pollen as a reward for pollinators would thus be more
expensive for these plants than producing ample nectar to attract them.
Birds might be a more effective pollinator because they
travel further than insects, increasing outcrosses and adding to the advantage
conferred on plants using them as preferred pollinators. In addition, it is
possible that birds act as defenders of plants against insect herbivory (Ford, Paton & Forde
1979).
Research has suggested that nectar-feeding birds have
evolved spatial memory that helps them avoid returning to flowers from which
they have fed until sufficient time has passed for the nectar supply to be
replenished (Burke & Fulham
2003),
demonstrating coevolution of traits of the partners in this mutualism.
The allure of cheating
For a mutualistic relationship to be of use to an
individual, the benefits must outweigh the costs [Figure 1]. When each partner
obtains a net benefit and barring any extrinsic disruptive events, the
mutualism should persist (Bshary & Bronstein
2004).
Nevertheless, most mutualisms are asymmetric, with one species benefiting more
than the other (Bronstein 1994). If this asymmetry becomes
pronounced over time due to environmental changes, the mutualism can transform
into a parasitic relationship (Hoeksema & Bruna
2000)
defined as occurring when the net cost exceeds the net benefit of the symbiosis.
The same two species in different environments can have a parasitic or a
mutualistic relationship, as with, for example, mycorrhizal symbioses under
different nutrient regimes (Johnson, NC, Graham
& Smith 1997).
The benefit to a symbiont of bypassing cooperation with its
partner and becoming parasitic is clear when assessed in terms of the cost it pays
in mutualism. To efficiently obtain the nutrients or services of its partner
without “payment” allows it to retain resources for itself. This process of
“cheating” is considered in a number of models of symbiosis that have been
developed, which will be reviewed later in this paper. Cheating is commonplace
among mutualisms (Bronstein 2001) and this can have a selective
pressure upon both the cheater to maintain or increase the benefit obtained as
well as on the cheated party to minimise the costs of the cheating (Bronstein, Alarcón
& Geber 2006).
Such asymmetric symbiosis thus need not automatically result in pure parasitism,
as evidenced by the persistence of mutualisms despite cheating (Ferriere et al. 2002).
Both pollinators and plants have been demonstrated to employ
cheating behaviour [Figure 2]. “Floral larceny” occurs when animal cheaters
obtain nectar without pollination, either as “nectar thieves” which enter
flowers in the usual manner but fail to brush against pollen, or as “nectar
robbers” which damage the flower corolla and extract nectar bypassing
reproductive parts (Irwin, Brody &
Waser 2001).
These exploiters tend to be opportunistic, rather than evolving from parasitic
lineages (Bronstein, Alarcón
& Geber 2006).
The effect on the fitness of the plants is not always negative, but sometimes
neutral or even positive (Irwin, Brody &
Waser 2001).
For example, a legitimate pollinator visiting a robbed flower will often travel
further in pursuit of more nectar, increasing the distance the pollen is taken
and thus improving outcrossing levels (Maloof & Inouye
2000).
Nevertheless, on balance, floral larceny tends on average have a negative
effect on the success of plant reproduction (Irwin, Brody &
Waser 2001).
Plants can cheat by way of unrewarding flowers. These
attract pollinators by deception (Renner 2006), which is particularly common
among orchids (Jersáková, Johnson
& Kindlmann 2006; Schiestl 2005). This deception can take a
number of forms. Sexual deception has been documented in the genus Cryptostylis (Gaskett &
Herberstein 2010).
The sole pollinator of Cryptostylis
is the male dupe wasp, Lissopimpla excelsa, Ichneumonidae. The male mistakes the Cryptostylis flower [Fig.3] for a
female, due to mimicry of both colour (Gaskett &
Herberstein 2010)
and pheromone (Schiestl, Peakall &
Mant 2004),
and attempts to mate with it. The subterfuge is so successful that the
wasp can even ejaculate during this process (Gaskett, Winnick &
Herberstein 2008).
Figure
3: Inflorescence of Cryptostylis subulata.
Scale: 1mm.
Image: Margaret Morgan. |
Another form of deception in rewardless flowers is floral
mimicry. Here, the plant species produces flowers that closely resemble the
rewarding flowers of another species (Johnson, SD 1994), creating convergent
phenotypes. The effectiveness of this strategy is strengthened by the physical
proximity of high-rewarding “magnet” plants (Johnson, SD et al.
2003).
Rewardless flowers can also attract pollinators by pseudoantagonism,
where the flower is “attacked” by an insect fooled into thinking it is another
insect encroaching on its territory. Pollen attaches to the insect, and is
transferred to another flower during subsequent territorial defensive attacks (Jersáková, Johnson
& Kindlmann 2006).
Others attract pollinators by resembling the ovipositioning sites
of various insects by mimicking the dung, carrion or fungi in which the larvae
grow (Jersáková, Johnson
& Kindlmann 2006).
There is strong selection pressure upon pollinators to learn
to avoid rewardless flowers and this in turn provides pressure on the plants to
finesse their deception. This mechanism is thought to be a driver in the high
level of species diversity among orchids, around one third of which pollinate
through deception (Cozzolino & Widmer
2005).
Less clear is what the overall advantage to rewardless
flower species might be for the cheating strategy. As previously noted, relatively,
nectar is not an expensive product for plants to make. Further, rewardless
plants generally have low pollinator visitor rates (Jersáková, Johnson
& Kindlmann 2006).
Rewardless flowers tend to occur in species where there are low population
densities and where a single visit is enough to remove all pollen from the
flower. Thus it is possible that the mechanism is sufficient to ensure higher
rates of cross-fertilisation than self-fertilisation, although there is as yet
no evidence suggesting that out-breeding is more common in deceptive orchids (Jersáková, Johnson
& Kindlmann 2006).
The resilience of
mutualisms
There is a significant asymmetry in cheating rates of plants
in comparison to their putative pollinators, with far more foraging insect
species displaying exploitation of plants than vice versa. This is thought to reflect the difference in mobility
between the partners, with plants being sedentary in contrast with highly
mobile insects. Mobility in insects increases the ability to locate suitable
alternative food sources or brood sites. Some plants have entirely dispensed
with the requirement of biotic pollinators, instead relying on
self-fertilisation or abiotic pollination through wind or water (Bronstein, Alarcón
& Geber 2006),
and it is possible that this change in mechanism is a result of growing loss of
reproductive fitness due to cheating. The interaction of symbionts evolves over
time, with new associations forming, while others are lost (Kiers et al. 2010). Overall, however, mutualism is
evolutionarily stable (Ferriere et al. 2002)
Resilient mutualisms tend to share certain characteristics (Kiers et al. 2010). Facultative interactions,
for example, allow participants more options in obtaining services and products
(Bronstein 1994). If the mutualism is
essential for short term survival, its absence will have profoundly disruptive
effects on the symbiont.
Protection from extreme changes in environment also lends
stability to mutualisms. Leaf cutter ants, for example, have sustained their
symbiosis with the fungus they farm for over 50-million years, and it has been
suggested that this is in part because of the protection of the ant nest
itself, where environmental conditions are kept in an optimal state (Mueller et al. 2005).
The strict control of symbionts by their partners to ensure
“loyalty” is considered a vital element for the persistence of some mutualisms,
and models of the methods by which this is policed are considered in the next
section of this review. Examples of this control are the propagation by African
fungus-growing termites of a single variant of its fungal “crop” (Aanen et al. 2009), and similar behaviour by the
leaf cutter ant Acromyrmex
octospinosus (Barke et al. 2010). It is suggested
that his limitation of the genetic diversity in the symbiont restricts its
ability to evolve the ability to cheat (Kiers et al. 2010).
Models of mutualism
A number of models have been developed or applied to explain
the maintenance of mutualisms and methods by which exploitation of the
relationship is minimised.
The Prisoner’s Dilemma, Iterated
Prisoner’s Dilemma and Tit for Tat.
The prisoner’s dilemma is a model derived from Game Theory,
which explores simplified options and their potential benefit or loss for the
participants. The iterated version of the problem is the same scenario multiply
repeated, and was formalised by Axelrod and Hamilton (1981). In its original formulation, it functions as
follows. Two suspects are arrested by police and interrogated separately. Neither
will confess. The police have insufficient evidence to charge them, and need at
least one to “grass” on the other. Each prisoner has two options:
· * Defect from each other (agreeing to give
evidence for the prosecution)
· * Cooperate with each other (remaining silent)
If one prisoner defects and the other cooperates, the first
prisoner will be released and the second will receive the maximum penalty of
ten years’ imprisonment. If the first prisoner defects and so does the other,
they will each receive five year sentences. If both cooperate (remain silent),
the police will not be able to prove their case, and each prisoner receives six
months on a minor charge.
Defecting is the best option for each prisoner, given that
their goal is to limit the length of time they will be imprisoned. With the
iterated version of the game, however, the prisoners’ are able to punish their
co-accused for previous choices. In this case, it was found using computer
modelling that the optimal strategy was “tit for tat”, in which a player
initially cooperates, but then makes a choice each time thereafter which
repeats the opponent’s last move. Under this scenario, both parties ultimately benefit
the most. This idea was applied as a possible mechanism explaining intraspecies
cooperation, (Axelrod & Hamilton
1981),
and has since been extended to explore interspecies cooperation. Unfortunately,
the model has failed to replicate what is seen in the natural world in
symbiotic relationships (Bshary & Bronstein
2004).
One explanation for this is that the model has an all-or-nothing structure
which does not reflect the complex gradients of real biological interactions (Hoeksema & Bruna
2000).
A multiscale approach that incorporates classes of individuals (species) and
population fluctuations into the iterated prisoner’s dilemma has been suggested
(Doebeli & Knowlton
1998).
This version allows for a range of possible responses, based on the variable
amount of benefit a symbiont is able to provide according to its resources. This
is compatible with Partner Fidelity Feedback (see below), and it appears to
predict at least some real-world phenomena with accuracy, such as strong
asymmetry in benefits given (Bshary & Bronstein
2004).
Biomarkets.
Biomarkets, or biological market theory, is another game
theoretic model, and uses as its basis the idea that mutualism can be analysed
as a trading of goods or commodities in a market where the parties differ in
their ease of producing the benefit sought by the other. Partner choice is an
element in this model, and it is posited that such choice is a mechanism by
which investment is made in the other symbiont, explaining the persistence of
mutualism. Failure to provide risks a cheater being abandoned by the choosing
partner (Bshary & Bronstein
2004; Noë 2001).
This is consistent with the host
sanctions model, below.
By-Product Mutualism
This is an unusual model, because it repudiates the concept
usually thought to be at the heart of mutualism: cooperation. By-product
mutualism suggests that the only benefits that a symbiont provides to its
partner are simply unwanted by-products of its own function. There is no cost
in such an arrangement (Brown 1983).
Host Sanctions and Partner Fidelity Feedback
Host Sanctions (HS) and Partner Fidelity Feedback (PFF) are
models which seek to explain how a mutualist responds to a cheating partner,
with the effect of ameliorating such cheating behaviour in the future (Bull & Rice 1991;
West et al. 2002).
They have been compared to two competing systems of criminal justice (Weyl et al. 2010): HS operates by punishment as
a deterrent against future bad behaviour (by selecting for “evolutionary
improvement” in the pool of potential symbionts). The analogy for PFF, by
contrast, is a more nuanced, non-retributive treatment of wrong-doers,
incorporating a sort of “harm minimisation” approach. In biological terms, this
operates by the wellbeing of one symbiont being adversely affected by the
exploitation of the other, leading to fewer rewards being physically able to be
forthcoming to the latter.
An analysis of these competing models has been undertaken
using economic contract theory (Weyl et al. 2010). Weyl et al. tested two commonly cited examples of mutualisms (yucca plants and yucca moths; and legume
plants and rhizobia bacteria) and concluded that contrary to the common
suggestion that these associations incorporate HS, they in fact demonstrate
PFF. This research provides a useful means by which to quantify factors
previously somewhat nebulous in PFF models.
Criticism of the
models
Bronstein makes substantial criticisms of many of the
current models (Bronstein 1994, 2001;
Bronstein, Alarcón & Geber 2006),
not least because the language of “cheating”, “temptation” and “punishment”
inadvertently implies an intention and subterfuge on the part of (usually)
non-sentient symbionts. Rather than the term “cheater” as suggested by previous
researchers (Soberon & Martinez
del Rio 1985),
she proposes “exploiter” and argues that without careful distinction between
types of exploiters, their evolutionary and ecological consequences will be
conflated (Bronstein 2001).
“Exploiter species”, according to this proposal, should be
divided into “pure exploiters” and “conditional exploiters”. The former entails
a pure strategy that encompasses life-long behaviour within a mutualistic
species. Sexual deception in orchids, described above, would fall into this
category. “Conditional exploiters”, by contrast, can and do act
mutualistically, depending on environmental conditions. This includes nectar
robbing insects which also can pollinate plants, and ant defenders which sometimes
attack their mutualists.
“Conditional exploiters” represent the majority of
“cheaters” in mutualistic relationships and are those which are most likely to
shift along the spectrum between complete mutualism and unilateral antagonism,
or parasitism.
Conclusion:
Mutualism is a fundamentally important element of evolution
and the development of biodiversity, acting as a driver for both radiation and
speciation. It sits on a transitional spectrum of interactions which ranges
from pure exploitation to balanced cooperation, and its position is never
static. It engages species from all kingdoms and all ecosystems and is
undeniably crucial in the evolution and survival of angiosperms, a group
pivotal for our species’ survival and that of many others.
While many mutualistic interactions have been identified and
to some extent empirically quantified, a single unifying theory explaining the
mechanisms leading to the maintenance of mutualisms is lacking to an extent
that is disproportionate to the undeniable importance of mutualisms in
evolution and ecology. Some of the current models seeking to explain the
exploitation of mutualisms suffer from insufficient regard to evolutionary origin,
and few succeed in accurately predicting actual behaviours.
Multiple challenges lie ahead in this field. Why, for
example, do some species act as exploiters in some environmental contexts and
mutualists in others? At what point will a symbiont abandon a mutualistic
relationship? How will climate change and increasing habitat fragmentation
impact upon the maintenance of mutualisms?
Theory in this area of biological study needs to meld more
completely with natural history and empirical observation, and with substantial
further research, these questions might be answered.
References
Aanen, DK, de Fine Licht, HH, Debets, AJM,
Kerstes, NAG, Hoekstra, RF & Boomsma, JJ 2009, 'High symbiont relatedness
stabilizes mutualistic cooperation in fungus-growing termites', Science, vol. 326, no. 5956, p. 1103.
Addicott, JF, Bronstein, J & Kjellberg, F 1990, 'Evolution of
mutualistic life-cycles: yucca moths and fig wasps', Genetics, evolution, and coordination of insect life cycles, pp.
143–61.
Atwell, B, Kriedemann, P & Turnbull, C 1999, Plants in Action: adaptation in nature, performance in cultivation,
MacMillan, Melbourne.
Axelrod, R & Hamilton, WD 1981, 'The evolution of cooperation', Science, vol. 211, no. 4489, p. 1390.
Barke, J, Seipke, RF, Gruschow, S, Heavens, D, Drou, N, Bibb, MJ, Goss,
RJM, Yu, DW & Hutchings, MI 2010, 'A mixed community of actinomycetes
produce multiple antibiotics for the fungus farming ant Acromyrmex
octospinosus', BMC biology, vol. 8,
no. 1, p. 109.
Bronstein, JL 1994, 'Our current understanding of mutualism', Quarterly Review of Biology, vol. 69,
no. 1, pp. 31-51.
—— 2001, 'The exploitation of mutualisms', Ecology Letters, vol. 4, no. 3, pp. 277-87.
Bronstein, JL, Alarcón, R & Geber, M 2006, 'The evolution of
plant–insect mutualisms', New Phytologist,
vol. 172, no. 3, pp. 412-28.
Brown, JL 1983, Cooperation: a
biologist’s dilemma, vol. 13, Advances in the Study of Behavior, Academic
Press, New York.
Bshary, R & Bronstein, JL 2004, 'Game Structures in Mutualistic
Interactions: What Can the Evidence Tell Us About the Kind of Models We Need?',
Advances in the Study of Behavior,
vol. 34, pp. 59-101.
Bull, JJ & Rice, WR 1991, 'Distinguishing mechanisms for the evolution
of co-operation', Journal of theoretical
biology, vol. 149, no. 1, pp. 63-74.
Burke, D & Fulham, BJ 2003, 'An evolved spatial memory bias in a
nectar-feeding bird?', Animal Behaviour,
vol. 66, no. 4, pp. 695-701.
Cozzolino, S & Widmer, A 2005, 'Orchid diversity: an evolutionary
consequence of deception?', Trends in
Ecology & Evolution, vol. 20, no. 9, pp. 487-94.
Darwin, C 1859, On the origin of
species by means of natural selection, or the preservation of favoured races in
the struggle for life, New York: D. Appleton.
Dawkins, R 2006, The selfish gene,
Oxford University Press, USA.
Doebeli, M & Knowlton, N 1998, 'The evolution of interspecific
mutualisms', Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 95, no. 15, p.
8676.
Fenster, CB, Armbruster, WS, Wilson, P, Dudash, MR & Thomson, JD
2004, 'Pollination syndromes and floral specialization', Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, vol. 35, pp.
375-403.
Ferriere, R, Bronstein, JL, Rinaldi, S, Law, R & Gauduchon, M 2002,
'Cheating and the evolutionary stability of mutualisms', Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological
Sciences, vol. 269, no. 1493, p. 773.
Ford, HA, Paton, DC & Forde, N 1979, 'Birds as pollinators of
Australian plants', New Zealand Journal
of Botany, vol. 17, pp. 509-19.
Gaskett, AC & Herberstein, ME 2010, 'Colour mimicry and sexual
deception by Tongue orchids (Cryptostylis)',
Naturwissenschaften, vol. 97, no. 1,
pp. 97-102.
Gaskett, AC, Winnick, CG & Herberstein, ME 2008, 'Orchid sexual
deceit provokes ejaculation', Natural
History, vol. 171, no. 6.
Grimaldi, DA & Engel, MS 2005, Evolution
of the Insects, Cambridge Univ Pr.
Hamilton, WD 1964, 'The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I and
II.', Journal of theoretical biology,
vol. 7, no. 1, pp. 17-52.
Hoeksema, JD & Bruna, EM 2000, 'Pursuing the big questions about
interspecific mutualism: a review of theoretical approaches', Oecologia, vol. 125, no. 3, pp. 321-30.
Huxley, TH 1888, 'The struggle for existence and its bearing upon
man.', Nineteenth Century, vol. 23,
pp. 163–86.
Irwin, RE, Brody, AK & Waser, NM 2001, 'The impact of floral
larceny on individuals, populations, and communities', Oecologia, vol. 129, no. 2, pp. 161-8.
Jandér, KC & Herre, EA 2010, 'Host sanctions and pollinator
cheating in the fig tree–fig wasp mutualism', Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol. 277,
no. 1687, p. 1481.
Jersáková, J, Johnson, SD & Kindlmann, P 2006, 'Mechanisms and
evolution of deceptive pollination in orchids', Biological Reviews, vol. 81, no. 02, pp. 219-35.
Johnson, NC, Graham, JH & Smith, FA 1997, 'Functioning of
mycorrhizal associations along the mutualism-parasitism continuum', New Phytologist, vol. 135, no. 4, pp.
575-86.
Johnson, SD 1994, 'Evidence for Batesian mimicry in a butterfly
pollinated orchid', Biological Journal of
the Linnean Society, vol. 53, no. 1, pp. 91-104.
Johnson, SD, Peter, CI, Nilsson, LA & Ågren, J 2003, 'Pollination
success in a deceptive orchid is enhanced by co-occurring rewarding magnet
plants', Ecology, vol. 84, no. 11,
pp. 2919-27.
Kiers, TE, Palmer, TM, Ives, AR, Bruno, JF & Bronstein, JL 2010,
'Mutualisms in a changing world: an evolutionary perspective', Ecology Letters, pp. no-no.
Kozo-Polyansky, BM 1924, Symbiogenesis:
a new principle of evolution., Harvard University Press, Cambridge,
Massachusetts.
Kropotkin, P 1902, 'Mutual Aid: A Factor of Evolution', in History of Economic Thought Books.
Labandeira, CC 1997, 'Insect mouthparts: ascertaining the paleobiology
of insect feeding strategies', Annual
Review of Ecology and Systematics, vol. 28, no. 1, pp. 153-93.
Maloof, JE & Inouye, DW 2000, 'Are nectar robbers cheaters or
mutualists?', Ecology, vol. 81, no.
10, pp. 2651-61.
Margulis, L 1967, 'On the origin of mitosing cells', Journal of theoretical biology, vol. 14,
no. 3, pp. 225-74.
Mueller, UG, Gerardo, NM, Aanen, DK, Six, DL & Schultz, TR 2005,
'The evolution of agriculture in insects', Annual
Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, vol. 36, no. 1, p. 563.
Noë, R 2001, Biological markets:
partner choice as the driving force behind the evolution of mutualisms,
Economics in nature: Social dilemmas, mate choice and biological markets,
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Paracer, S & Ahmadjian, V 2000, Symbiosis:
an introduction to biological associations, Oxford University Press, USA.
Pirozynski, KA & Malloch, DW 1975, 'The origin of land plants: a
matter of mycotrophism', BioSystems,
vol. 6, no. 3, pp. 153-64.
Renner, SS 2006, 'Rewardless flowers in the angiosperms and the role of
insect cognition in their evolution', in NM Waser & J Ollerton (eds), Plant-pollinator interactions: from
specialization to generalization, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp.
123–44.
Sapp, J 1994, Evolution by
association : a history of symbiosis, Oxford University Press, New York.
Schiestl, FP 2005, 'On the success of a swindle: pollination by
deception in orchids', Naturwissenschaften,
vol. 92, no. 6, pp. 255-64.
Schiestl, FP, Peakall, R & Mant, J 2004, 'Chemical communication in
the sexually deceptive orchid genus Cryptostylis', Botanical Journal of the Linnean Society, vol. 144, no. 2, pp.
199-205.
Soberon, JM & Martinez del Rio, C 1985, Cheating and taking advantage in mutualistic interactions, The
biology of mutualism Oxford, New York.
Spencer, H 1904, The principles
of biology, D. Appleton and company.
Stadler, B & Dixon, AFG 2008, Mutualism:
ants and their insect partners, Cambridge Univ Pr.
Tarachai, Y, Compton, SG & Trisonthi, C 2008, 'The benefits of
pollination for a fig wasp', Symbiosis,
vol. 45, no. 1-3, pp. 29-32.
West, SA, Kiers, ET, Pen, I & Denison, RF 2002, 'Sanctions and
mutualism stability: when should less beneficial mutualists be tolerated?', Journal of Evolutionary Biology, vol.
15, no. 5, pp. 830-7.
Weyl, EG, Frederickson, ME, Yu, DW & Pierce, NE 2010, 'Economic
contract theory tests models of mutualism', Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences.
Wilson, GWT, Rice, CW, Rillig, MC, Springer, A & Hartnett, DC 2009,
'Soil aggregation and carbon sequestration are tightly correlated with the
abundance of arbuscular mycorrhizal fungi: results from long term field
experiments', Ecology Letters, vol.
12, no. 5, pp. 452-61.