Sleep

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Jump to: navigation, search
This article is about sleep in humans. For non-human sleep, see Sleep (non-human). For other uses, see Sleep (disambiguation).
"Sleep architecture", "Waking up", "Asleep", and "Slept" redirect here. For other uses, see Waking Up (disambiguation), Asleep (disambiguation), and SLEPT analysis.
Sleep is associated with a state of muscle relaxation and reduced perception of environmental stimuli.

Sleep is a naturally recurring state of mind and body characterized by altered consciousness, relatively inhibited sensory activity, inhibition of nearly all voluntary muscles, and reduced interactions with surroundings.[1] It is distinguished from wakefulness by a decreased ability to react to stimuli, but is more easily reversed than the state of being comatose. Sleep occurs in repeating periods, in which the body alternates between two highly distinct modes known as non-REM and REM sleep. Although REM stands for "rapid eye movement", sleep affects other brain-body functions, including virtual paralysis of the body.

During sleep, most systems are in an anabolic state, helping to restore the immune, nervous, skeletal, and muscular systems. The internal circadian clock promotes sleep daily at night. However, sleep patterns vary among individuals. In the last century, artificial light has substantially altered sleep timing in industrialized countries.[2]

The diverse purposes and mechanisms of sleep are the subject of substantial ongoing research.[3] Sleep seems to assist with improvements in the body and mind. Research in the 21st century is investigating whether sleep is a period of maintenance for removing metabolic waste compounds from the brain.[4]

A well-known feature of sleep is the dream, an experience typically recounted in narrative form, which resembles waking life while in progress, but which usually can later be distinguished as fantasy. Sleep is sometimes confused with unconsciousness, but is quite different in terms of the thought process.

Humans may suffer from various sleep disorders, including

Physiology[edit]

Main article: Neuroscience of sleep
An artist's creative illustration depicting REM sleep.

Sleep is divided into two broad types: rapid eye movement (REM sleep) and non-rapid eye movement (non-REM or NREM sleep). REM and non-REM sleep are so different that physiologists classify them as distinct behavioral states. Dreams (or nightmares) occur during REM sleep. REM sleep is associated with desynchronized and fast brain waves, loss of muscle tone,[1] and suspension of homeostasis[citation needed]. NREM is considered to be deep sleep (the deepest part of NREM is called slow wave sleep); it shows no prominent eye movement or muscle paralysis.

Sleep occurs in cycles of approximately 90 minutes. This rhythm is called the ultradian sleep cycle.[5] Sleep proceeds in cycles of NREM and REM, normally in that order and usually four or five such cycles per night. The American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) divides NREM into three stages: N1, N2, and N3, the last of which is also called delta sleep or slow-wave sleep.[6] The whole period normally proceeds in the order: N1 → N2 → N3 → N2 → REM. REM sleep occurs as a person returns to stage 2 or 1 from a deep sleep.[1] An adult reaches REM approximately every 90 minutes; REM sleep usually lasts for longer during latter half of sleep than in the early part of the sleep episode. There is a greater amount of deep sleep (stage N3) earlier in the night, while the proportion of REM sleep increases in the two cycles just before natural awakening.

Key physiological indicators in sleep include EEG of brain waves, electrooculography (EOG) of eye movements, and electromyography (EMG) of skeletal muscle activity. Simultaneous collection of these measurements is called polysomnography, and can be performed in a specialized sleep laboratory.[7]

Sleep increases the sensory threshold. In other words, sleeping persons perceive fewer stimuli. However, they can generally still respond to loud noises and other salient sensory events.[8]

Awakening[edit]

Awakening can mean the end of sleep, or simply a moment to survey the environment and readjust body position before falling back asleep. Sleepers typically awaken from slow-wave sleep, soon after the end of a REM phase or sometimes in the middle of REM. Internal circadian indicators, along with successful reduction of homeostatic sleep need, typically bring about awakening and the end of the sleep episode.[9]

Today, many humans wake up with an alarm clock.[10] (Some people, however, can reliably wake themselves up at a specific time with no need for an alarm.)[9] Many sleep quite differently on workdays versus days off, a pattern which can lead to chronic circadian desynchronization.[11][10] Many people regularly look at television and other screens before going to bed, a factor which may exacerbate this mass circadian disruption.[12]

Awakening involves heightened electrical activation in the brain, beginning with the thalamus and spreading throughout the cortex.[9]

During a night's sleep, a small portion is usually spent in a waking state. As measured by electroencephalography, young females are awake for 0–1% of the larger sleeping period; young males are awake for 0–2%. In adults, wakefulness increases, especially in later cycles. One study found 3% awake time in the first ninety-minute sleep cycle, 8% in the second, 10% in the third, 12% in the fourth, and 13–14% in the fifth. Most of this awake time occurred shortly after REM sleep.[9]

Scientific studies on sleep have shown that sleep stage at awakening is an important factor in amplifying sleep inertia.[13] Alarm clocks involving sleep stage monitoring appeared on the market in 2005.[14] Using sensing technologies such as EEG electrodes or accelerometers, these alarm clocks are supposed to wake people only from light sleep.

Circadian timing[edit]

Sleep timing is controlled by the circadian clock, sleep-wake homeostasis, and in humans, within certain bounds, willed behavior. The circadian clock—an inner timekeeping, temperature-fluctuating, enzyme-controlling device—works in tandem with these other mechanisms. Circadian timing, known as process C, is cyclical, based on the time of day; sleep-wake homeostasis, or process S, operates on a more absolute scale. The circadian process is thought to counteract the homeostatic drive for sleep during the day (in diurnal animals) and to enable it at night.[15][11]

Humans are also influenced by aspects of social time: the hours when other people are awake, the hours when work is required, the time on the clock, etc. Time zones, standard times used to unify the timing for people in the same area, correspond only approximately to the natural rising and setting of the sun. The approximate nature of the timezone can be shown with China, a country which used to span five time zones and now uses only one (UTC +8).[10]

Circadian clock[edit]

Main article: Circadian rhythm
The human "biological clock"

Biologically, the most important human circadian clock currently known to science is a dense cluster of neurons in the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN), a part of the brain directly above the optic chiasm, where it receives retinohypothalamic tract projections from specialized ganglion cells that synchronize it with the light dark cycle. This clock measures the time of day, primarily based on input from outside light signals. An organism whose circadian clock exhibits a regular rhythm corresponding to outside signals is said to be entrained; the rhythm so established persists even if the outside signals suddenly disappear. If you take an entrained human and put them in a bunker with constant light (or darkness), they will continue to experience rhythmic increases and decreases of body temperature and melatonin, on a period which slightly exceeds 24 hours. Scientists refer to such conditions as free-running of the circadian rhythm. (Under natural conditions, light signals regularly adjust this period downward, so that it corresponds better with the exact 24 hours of an Earth day.)[10][16][17] The SCN has the ability to synchronize the intrinsic time keeping capabilities of other tissue, contradictory to the previously accepted consensus in the field that the SCN was the only rhythmic tissue. The SCN creates the rhythmic pattern through a feedback loop of protein transcription. The cycle involves two phases, the transcription of the Period (gene) and the Cryptochrome gene initiated by CLOCK, ARNTL, and NPAS2. The accumulation of PER and CRY result in the inhibition of CLOCK, ARNTL and NPAS2, where over 24 hours the decay of PER/CRY inhibition results in the reinitiating of the cycle. Light entrainment occurs through retinohypothalamic tract induced expression of PER.[18]

The clock exerts constant influence on the body, effecting continuous sinusoidal oscillation of body temperature between roughly 36.2 °C and 37.2 °C.[17][19] The suprachiasmatic nucleus itself shows conspicuous oscillation activity, which intensifies during subjective day (i.e., the part of the rhythm corresponding with daytime, whether accurately or not) and drops to almost nothing during subjective night.[20] The circadian pacemaker in the suprachiasmatic nucleus has a direct neural connection to the pineal gland, which releases the hormone melatonin at night.[20] Melatonin is an important circadian indicator but its mechanisms of action are not well understood. Cortisol levels typically rise throughout the night, peak in the awakening hours, and diminish during the day.[21][22] Circadian prolactin secretion begins in the late afternoon, especially in women, and is subsequently augmented by sleep-induced secretion, to peak in the middle of the night. Circadian rhythm exerts some influence on the nighttime secretion of growth hormone.[21]

The circadian rhythm influences the ideal timing of a restorative sleep episode.[10][23] Sleepiness increases during the night. REM sleep occurs more during the low part (i.e., near body temperature minimum) of the circadian cycle, whereas slow-wave sleep occurs relatively independently of circadian time.[17]

The internal circadian clock is profoundly influenced by changes in light, since these are its main clues about what time it is. Exposure to even small amounts of light during the night can suppress melatonin secretion, increase body temperature, and increase cognitive ability. Short pulses of light, at the right moment in the circadian cycle, can significantly 'reset' the internal clock.[19] Blue light, in particular, exerts the strongest effect,[11] leading to concerns that electronic media use before bed may interfere with sleep.

Modern humans often find themselves desynchronized from their internal circadian clock, due to the requirements of work (especially night shifts), long-distance travel, and the influence of widespread indoor lighting.[17] Even if they have sleep debt, or feel sleepy, people can have difficulty staying asleep at the peak of their circadian cycle. Conversely they can have difficulty waking up in the trough of the cycle.[9] A healthy young adult entrained to the sun will (during most of the year) fall asleep a few hours after sunset, experience body temperature minimum at 6AM, and wake up a few hours after sunrise.[17]

Distribution[edit]

In polyphasic sleep, an organism sleeps at multiple times during a 24-hour cycle. Monophasic sleep occurs all at once. Under experimental conditions, humans tend to alternate more frequently between sleep and wakefulness (i.e., exhibit more polyphasic sleep) if they have nothing better to do.[17] Given a 14-hour period of darkness in experimental conditions, humans tended towards bimodal sleep, with two sleep periods concentrated at the beginning and at the end of the dark time. Bimodal sleep in humans was more common before the industrial revolution.[22]

Different characteristic sleep patterns, such as the familiarly so-called "early bird" and "night owl", are called chronotypes. Genetics and sex have some influence on chronotype, but so do habits. Chronotype is also liable to change over the course of a person's lifetime. Seven-year-olds are better disposed to wake up early in the morning than are fifteen-year-olds.[11][10] Chronotypes far outside the normal range are called circadian rhythm sleep disorders.[24]

Naps[edit]

Main article: Nap

The siesta habit has recently been associated with a 37% lower coronary mortality, possibly due to reduced cardiovascular stress mediated by daytime sleep.[25] Short naps at mid-day and mild evening exercise were found to be effective for improved sleep, cognitive tasks, and mental health in elderly people.[26]

Quality[edit]

The quality of sleep may be evaluated from an objective and a subjective point of view. Objective sleep quality refers to how difficult it is for a person to fall asleep and remain in a sleeping state, and how many times they wake up during a single night. Poor sleep quality disrupts the cycle of transition between the different stages of sleep.[27] Subjective sleep quality in turn refers to a sense of being rested and regenerated after awaking from sleep. A study by A. Harvey et al. (2002) found that insomniacs were more demanding in their evaluations of sleep quality than individuals who had no sleep problems.[28]

Genetics[edit]

It is hypothesized that a considerable amount of sleep-related behavior, such as when and how long a person needs to sleep, is regulated by genetics. Researchers have discovered some evidence that seems to support this assumption.[29] Monozygotic (identical) but not dizygotic (fraternal) twins tend to have similar sleep habits. Neurotransmitters, molecules whose production can be traced to specific genes, are one genetic influence on sleep which can be analyzed. And the circadian clock has its own set of genes.[30] ABCC9 is one gene found which influences the duration of human sleep.[31]

Sleep duration is affected by the gene DEC2. People with a certain DEC2 mutation sleep two hours less than normal. The gene also affects the sleep patterns of mice, and likely does so for all mammals.[32][33]

Recent studies show three genetic variants associated with sleep duration: the most strongly associated variant occurs near PAX 8, with an average of 2.6 minute per allele change in sleep duration; a variant located downstream of VRK2 ( vaccinia related Kinase 2) gene, that has an average per allele effect of 2.0 minutes on sleep duration; and a variant located upstream of VRK2 gene, that has an average per allele effect of 1.6 minutes on sleep duration.[34]

Sleep homeostasis, deprivation and optimization[edit]

Generally speaking, the longer an organism is awake, the more it feels a need to sleep, and this driver of sleep is called Process S. The balance between sleeping and waking is regulated by a process called homeostasis. Induced or perceived lack of sleep is commonly called sleep deprivation.

Process S is driven by the depletion of glycogen and accumulation of adenosine in the forebrain that disinhibits the Ventrolateral preoptic nucleus, allowing for inhibition of the ascending reticular activating system.[35]

Sleep deprivation tends to cause slower brain waves in the frontal cortex, shortened attention span, higher anxiety, impaired memory, and a grouchy mood. Conversely, a well-rested organism tends to have improved memory and mood.[36]

In humans, sleep deprivation has been studied up to 11 days, during which subjects are more likely to gain weight.[citation needed]

Sleep debt[edit]

Main article: Sleep debt

Sleep debt is the effect of not getting enough sleep; a large debt causes fatigue, both mental and physical.

Sleep debt results in diminished abilities to perform high-level cognitive functions. Neurophysiological and functional imaging studies have demonstrated that frontal regions of the brain are particularly responsive to homeostatic sleep pressure.[37]

Scientists do not agree on how much sleep debt it is possible to accumulate; whether it is accumulated against an individual's average sleep or some other benchmark; nor on whether the prevalence of sleep debt among adults has changed appreciably in the industrialized world in recent decades. Sleep debt does show some evidence of being cumulative. Subjectively, however, humans seem to reach maximum sleepiness after 30 hours of waking.[17]

It is likely that children are sleeping less than previously in Western societies.[38]

One neurochemical indicator of sleep debt is adenosine, a neurotransmitter that inhibits many of the bodily processes associated with wakefulness. Adenosine levels increase in the cortex and basal forebrain during prolonged wakefulness and decrease during the sleep-recovery period, potentially acting as a homeostatic regulator of sleep.[39][40] Coffee and caffeine temporarily block the effect of adenosine, prolong sleep latency, and reduce total sleep time and quality.[41]

Duration[edit]

Homeostatic sleep propensity (the need for sleep as a function of the amount of time elapsed since the last adequate sleep episode) must be balanced against the circadian element for satisfactory sleep.[42] Along with corresponding messages from the circadian clock, this tells the body it needs to sleep.[43] Sleep offset (awakening) is primarily determined by circadian rhythm. A person who regularly awakens at an early hour will generally not be able to sleep much later than his or her normal waking time, even if moderately sleep-deprived[citation needed].

Adult humans[edit]

The main health effects of sleep deprivation,[44] indicating impairment of normal maintenance by sleep.

The optimal amount of sleep is not a meaningful concept unless the timing of that sleep is seen in relation to an individual's circadian rhythms. A person's major sleep episode is relatively inefficient and inadequate when it occurs at the "wrong" time of day; one should be asleep at least six hours before the lowest body temperature.[45] The timing is correct when the following two circadian markers occur after the middle of the sleep episode and before awakening:[46] maximum concentration of the hormone melatonin, and minimum core body temperature.

Human sleep needs vary by age and amongst individuals, and sleep is considered to be adequate when there is no daytime sleepiness or dysfunction. Moreover, self-reported sleep duration is only moderately correlated with actual sleep time as measured by actigraphy,[47] and those affected with sleep state misperception may typically report having slept only four hours despite having slept a full eight hours.[48]

A University of California, San Diego psychiatry study of more than one million adults found that people who live the longest self-report sleeping for six to seven hours each night.[49] Another study of sleep duration and mortality risk in women showed similar results.[50] Other studies[which?] show that "sleeping more than 7 to 8 hours per day has been consistently associated with increased mortality," though this study suggests the cause is probably other factors such as depression and socioeconomic status, which would correlate statistically.[51]

Researchers at the University of Warwick and University College London have found that lack of sleep can more than double the risk of death from cardiovascular disease, but that too much sleep can also be associated with a doubling of the risk of death, though not primarily from cardiovascular disease.[52]

Professor Francesco Cappuccio said, "Short sleep has been shown to be a risk factor for weight gain, hypertension, and Type 2 diabetes, sometimes leading to mortality; but in contrast to the short sleep-mortality association, it appears that no potential mechanisms by which long sleep could be associated with increased mortality have yet been investigated. Some candidate causes for this include depression, low socioeconomic status, and cancer-related fatigue... In terms of prevention, our findings indicate that consistently sleeping around seven hours per night is optimal for health, and a sustained reduction may predispose to ill health."[clarification needed]

Nevertheless, some new studies have identified links between too little or too much sleep with metabolic disorders such as obesity and type2 diabetes. There was a strong genetic correlation between under sleeping and BMI (Body mass index), but not type2 diabetes. There was also a genetic correlation between oversleeping and both BMI and type2 diabetes. However, cause-and-effect is not easily determined, because of multiple confounding factors affecting sleep patterns and disease risk.[34][53][54]

Furthermore, sleep difficulties are closely associated with psychiatric disorders such as depression, alcoholism, and bipolar disorder.[55] Up to 90% of adults with depression are found to have sleep difficulties. Dysregulation found on EEG includes disturbances in sleep continuity, decreased delta sleep and altered REM patterns with regard to latency, distribution across the night and density of eye movements.[56]

Children[edit]

By the time infants reach the age of two, their brain size has reached 90 percent of an adult-sized brain;[57] a majority of this brain growth has occurred during the period of life with the highest rate of sleep. The hours that children spend asleep influence their ability to perform on cognitive tasks.[58][59] Children who sleep through the night and have few night waking episodes have higher cognitive attainments and easier temperaments than other children.[59][60][61]

Sleep also influences language development. To test this, researchers taught infants a faux language and observed their recollection of the rules for that language.[62] Infants who slept within four hours of learning the language could remember the language rules better, while infants who stayed awake longer did not recall those rules as well. There is also a relationship between infants' vocabulary and sleeping: infants who sleep longer at night at 12 months have better vocabularies at 26 months.[61]

Recommendations[edit]

Children need many hours of sleep per day in order to develop and function properly: up to 18 hours for newborn babies, with a declining rate as a child ages.[43] Early in 2015, after a two-year study,[63] the National Sleep Foundation in the US announced newly revised recommendations as shown in the table below.

Age and condition Sleep Needs
Newborns (0–3 months) 14 to 17 hours[63]
Infants (4–11 months) 12 to 15 hours[63]
Toddlers (1–2 years) 11 to 14 hours[63]
Preschoolers (3–4 years) 10 to 13 hours[63]
School-age children (5–12 years)       9 to 11 hours[63]
Teenagers (13–17 years)   8 to 10 hours[63][64]
Adults (18–64 years)   7 to 9 hours[63]
Older Adults (65 years and over)   7 to 8 hours[63][65]

Ontogenesis[edit]

According to the ontogenetic hypothesis of REM sleep, the activity occurring during neonatal REM sleep (or active sleep) seems to be particularly important to the developing organism.[66] Studies investigating the effects of deprivation of active sleep have shown that deprivation early in life can result in behavioral problems, permanent sleep disruption, decreased brain mass,[67] and an abnormal amount of neuronal cell death.[68]

REM sleep appears to be important for development of the brain. REM sleep occupies the majority of time of sleep of infants, who spend most of their time sleeping. Among different species, the more immature the baby is born, the more time it spends in REM sleep. Proponents also suggest that REM-induced muscle inhibition in the presence of brain activation exists to allow for brain development by activating the synapses, yet without any motor consequences that may get the infant in trouble. Additionally, REM deprivation results in developmental abnormalities later in life. However, this does not explain why older adults still need REM sleep.[69]

Memory processing[edit]

Scientists have shown numerous ways in which sleep is related to memory. In a study conducted by Turner, Drummond, Salamat, and Brown (2007), working memory was shown to be affected by sleep deprivation. Working memory is important because it keeps information active for further processing and supports higher-level cognitive functions such as decision making, reasoning, and episodic memory. The study allowed 18 women and 22 men to sleep only 26 minutes per night over a four-day period. Subjects were given initial cognitive tests while well-rested, and then were tested again twice a day during the four days of sleep deprivation. On the final test, the average working memory span of the sleep-deprived group had dropped by 38% in comparison to the control group.[70]

The relation between working memory and sleep can also be explored by testing how working memory works during sleep. Daltrozzo, Claude, Tillmann, Bastuji, and Perrin,[71] using Event-Related Potentials to the perception of sentences during sleep showed that working memory for linguistic information is partially preserved during sleep with a smaller capacity compared to wake.

Memory seems to be affected differently by certain stages of sleep such as REM and slow-wave sleep (SWS). In one study, multiple groups of human subjects were used: wake control groups and sleep test groups. Sleep and wake groups were taught a task and were then tested on it, both on early and late nights, with the order of nights balanced across participants. When the subjects' brains were scanned during sleep, hypnograms revealed that SWS was the dominant sleep stage during the early night, representing around 23% on average for sleep stage activity. The early-night test group performed 16% better on the declarative memory test than the control group. During late-night sleep, which entails more time spent in REM, test group performed 25% better on the procedural memory test than the control group. This suggests that procedural memory benefits from late, REM-rich sleep, whereas declarative memory benefits from early, slow wave-rich sleep.[72]

A study has also been done involving direct current stimulation to the prefrontal cortex to increase the amount of slow oscillations during SWS. The direct current stimulation greatly enhanced word-pair retention the following day, giving evidence that SWS plays a large role in the consolidation of episodic memories.[73]

The different studies suggest that there is a correlation between sleep and the complex functions of memory. Harvard sleep researchers Saper[74] and Stickgold[75] point out that an essential part of memory and learning consists of nerve cell dendrites' sending of information to the cell body to be organized into new neuronal connections. This process demands that no external information is presented to these dendrites, and it is suggested that this may be why it is during sleep that memories and knowledge are solidified and organized.

Recent studies examining gene expression and evolutionary increases in brain size offer complimentary support for the role of sleep in the mammalian memory consolidation theory. Evolutionary advances in the size of the mammalian amygdala, (a brain structure active during sleep and involved in memory processing), are also associated with increases in NREM sleep durations.[76] Likewise, nighttime gene expression differs from daytime expression and specifically targets genes thought to be involved in memory consolidation and brain plasticity.[77]

Dreaming[edit]

Main article: Dream
Bronze statue of Eros sleeping, 3rd century BC–early 1st century AD

During sleep, especially REM sleep, people tend to have dreams: elusive first-person experiences, which, despite their frequently bizarre qualities, seem realistic while in progress. Dreams can seamlessly incorporate elements within a person's mind that would not normally go together. They can include apparent sensations of all types, especially vision and movement.[78]

Dreams can also be suppressed or encouraged; using anti-depressants, acetaminophen, ibuprofen, or alcoholic beverages is thought to potentially suppress dreams, whereas melatonin may have the ability to encourage them.[79]

People have proposed many hypotheses about the functions of dreaming. Sigmund Freud postulated that dreams are the symbolic expression of frustrated desires that have been relegated to the unconscious mind, and he used dream interpretation in the form of psychoanalysis in attempting to uncover these desires.[80]

Freud's work concerns the psychological role of dreams, which does not exclude any physiological role they may have. Recent research claims that sleep has the overall role of consolidation and organization of synaptic connections formed during learning and experience.[81] As such, Freud's work is not ruled out. Nevertheless, Freud's research has been expanded on, especially with regard to the organization and consolidation of recent memory.

While penile erections during sleep are commonly believed to indicate dreams with sexual content, they are not more frequent during sexual dreams than they are during nonsexual dreams.[82] The parasympathetic nervous system experiences increased activity during REM sleep which may cause erection of the penis or clitoris. In males, 80% to 95% of REM sleep is normally accompanied by partial to full penile erection, while only about 12% of men's dreams contain sexual content.[83]

John Allan Hobson and Robert McCarley propose that dreams are caused by the random firing of neurons in the cerebral cortex during the REM period. Neatly, this theory helps explain the irrationality of the mind during REM periods, as, according to this theory, the forebrain then creates a story in an attempt to reconcile and make sense of the nonsensical sensory information presented to it. This would explain the odd nature of many dreams.[84]

Disorders[edit]

Insomnia[edit]

See also: Insomnia

Insomnia, a dyssomnia, is a general term describing difficulty falling asleep and staying asleep. Insomnia is the most common sleep problem, with many adults reporting occasional insomnia, and 10–15% reporting a chronic condition.[85] Insomnia can have many different causes, including psychological stress, a poor sleep environment, an inconsistent sleep schedule, or excessive mental or physical stimulation in the hours before bedtime. Insomnia is often treated through behavioral changes like keeping a regular sleep schedule, avoiding stimulating or stressful activities before bedtime, and cutting down on stimulants such as caffeine. The sleep environment may be improved by installing heavy drapes to shut out all sunlight, and keeping computers, televisions and work materials out of the sleeping area.

A 2010 review of published scientific research suggested that exercise generally improves sleep for most people, and helps sleep disorders such as insomnia. The optimum time to exercise may be 4 to 8 hours before bedtime, though exercise at any time of day is beneficial, with the exception of heavy exercise taken shortly before bedtime, which may disturb sleep. However, there is insufficient evidence to draw detailed conclusions about the relationship between exercise and sleep.[86] Sleeping medications such as Ambien and Lunesta are an increasingly popular treatment for insomnia. Although these nonbenzodiazepine medications are generally believed to be better and safer than earlier generations of sedatives, they have still generated some controversy and discussion regarding side-effects. White noise appears to be a promising treatment for insomnia.[87]

Obstructive sleep apnea[edit]

Obstructive sleep apnea is a condition in which major pauses in breathing occur during sleep, disrupting the normal progression of sleep and often causing other more severe health problems. Apneas occur when the muscles around the patient's airway relax during sleep, causing the airway to collapse and block the intake of oxygen.[88] Obstructive sleep apnea is more common than central sleep apnea.[89] As oxygen levels in the blood drop, the patient then comes out of deep sleep in order to resume breathing. When several of these episodes occur per hour, sleep apnea rises to a level of seriousness that may require treatment.

Diagnosing sleep apnea usually requires a professional sleep study performed in a sleep clinic, because the episodes of wakefulness caused by the disorder are extremely brief and patients usually do not remember experiencing them. Instead, many patients simply feel tired after getting several hours of sleep and have no idea why. Major risk factors for sleep apnea include chronic fatigue, old age, obesity and snoring.

Other disorders[edit]

Sleep disorders include narcolepsy, periodic limb movement disorder (PLMD), restless leg syndrome (RLS), upper airway resistance syndrome (UARS), and the circadian rhythm sleep disorders. Fatal familial insomnia, or FFI, an extremely rare genetic disease with no known treatment or cure, is characterized by increasing insomnia as one of its symptoms; ultimately sufferers of the disease stop sleeping entirely, before dying of the disease.[90]

Somnambulism, known as sleep walking, is also a common sleeping disorder, especially among children. In somnambulism the individual gets up from his/her sleep and wanders around while still sleeping.[91]

Older people may be more easily awakened by disturbances in the environment[92] and may to some degree lose the ability to consolidate sleep.

Effect of food and drugs on sleep[edit]

Hypnotics[edit]

  • Nonbenzodiazepine hypnotics such as eszopiclone (Lunesta), zaleplon (Sonata), and zolpidem (Ambien) are commonly used as sleep aids prescribed by doctors to treat forms of insomnia. Nonbenzodiazepines are the most commonly prescribed and over-the-counter sleep aids used worldwide and have been greatly growing in use since the 1990s. They target the GABAA receptor.
  • Benzodiazepines target the GABAA receptor also, and as such, they are commonly used sleep aids as well, though benzodiazepines have been found to decrease REM sleep.[93]
  • Antihistamines, such as diphenhydramine (Benadryl) and doxylamine (found in various OTC medicines, such as NyQuil)
  • Alcohol (ethanol) – Often, people start drinking alcohol in order to get to sleep (alcohol is initially a sedative and will cause somnolence, encouraging sleep). However, being addicted to alcohol can lead to disrupted sleep, because alcohol has a rebound effect later in the night. As a result, there is strong evidence linking alcoholism and forms of insomnia.[94] Alcohol also reduces REM sleep.[93]
  • Barbiturates cause drowsiness and have actions similar to alcohol in that they have a rebound effect and inhibit REM sleep, so they are not used as a long-term sleep aid.
  • Melatonin is a naturally occurring hormone that regulates sleepiness. It is made in the brain, where tryptophan is converted into serotonin and then into melatonin, which is released at night by the pineal gland to induce and maintain sleep. Melatonin supplementation may be used as a sleep aid, both as a hypnotic and as a chronobiotic (see phase response curve, PRC).[95]
  • Siesta and the "post-lunch dip" – Many people have a temporary drop in alertness in the early afternoon, commonly known as the "post-lunch dip." While a large meal can make a person feel sleepy, the post-lunch dip is mostly an effect of the circadian clock. People naturally feel most sleepy at two times of the day about 12 hours apart—for example, at 2:00 a.m. and 2:00 p.m. At those two times, the body clock "kicks in." At about 2 p.m. (14:00), it overrides the homeostatic buildup of sleep debt, allowing several more hours of wakefulness. At about 2 a.m. (02:00), with the daily sleep debt paid off, it "kicks in" again to ensure a few more hours of sleep.
  • Tryptophan – The amino acid tryptophan is a building block of proteins. It has been claimed to contribute to sleepiness, since it is a precursor of the neurotransmitter serotonin, involved in sleep regulation. However, no solid data have ever linked modest dietary changes in tryptophan to changes in sleep.
  • Cannabis – Some people use cannabis to induce sleepiness. Users often report relaxation and drowsiness. It has been shown that tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), the principal psychoactive constituent in cannabis, reduces the amount of REM sleep.[96] Frequent users often report being unable to recall their dreams.

Stimulants[edit]

  • Amphetamine (dextroamphetamine, and a related, slightly more powerful drug methamphetamine, etc.) are used to treat narcolepsy. Their most common effects are anxiety, insomnia, stimulation, increased alertness, and decreased hunger.
  • Caffeine is a stimulant that works by slowing the action of the hormones in the brain that cause somnolence, particularly by acting as an antagonist at adenosine receptors. Effective dosage is individual, in part dependent on prior usage. It can cause a rapid reduction in alertness as it wears off.
  • Cocaine and crack cocaine – Studies on cocaine have shown its effects to be mediated through the circadian rhythm system.[97] This may be related to the onset of hypersomnia (oversleeping) in regard to "cocaine-induced sleep disorder."[98]
  • MDMA, including similar drugs like MDA, MMDA, or bk-MDMA – The class of drugs called empathogen-entactogens keep users awake with intense euphoria.
  • Methylphenidate – Commonly known by the brand names Ritalin and Concerta, methylphenidate is similar in action to amphetamine and cocaine; its chemical composition more closely resembles that of cocaine.
  • Other analeptic drugs like Modafinil and Armodafinil are prescribed to treat narcolepsy, idiopathic hypersomnia, shift work sleep disorder, and other conditions causing excessive daytime sleepiness. The precise mechanism of these central nervous system (CNS) stimulants is not known, but they have been shown to increase both the release of monoamines and levels of hypothalamic histamine, thereby promoting wakefulness.

Nutritional effects on sleep[edit]

Dietary and nutritional choices affect sleep duration and quality. Research is being conducted in an attempt to discover what kinds of nutritional choices result in better sleep quality.

A study in the Western Journal of Nursing Research in 2011[99] compared how sleep quality was affected by four different diets: a high-protein diet, a high-fat diet, a high-carbohydrate diet, and a control diet. Results indicated that the diets high in protein resulted in fewer wakeful episodes during night-time sleep. The high carbohydrate diet was linked to much shorter periods of quiescent or restful sleep. These results suggest that ingested nutrients do play a role in determining sleep quality. Another investigation published in Nutrition Research in 2012[100] examined the effects of various combinations of dietary choices in regard to sleep. Although it is difficult to determine one perfect diet for sleep enhancement, this study indicated that a variety of micro and macro nutrients are needed to maintain levels of healthful and restful sleep. A varied diet containing fresh fruits and vegetables, low-fat proteins, and whole grains can be the best nutritional option for individuals seeking to improve the quality of their sleep.

Impact on culture[edit]

Anthropology[edit]

Research suggests that sleep patterns vary significantly across cultures.[101][102] The most striking differences are between societies that have plentiful sources of artificial light and ones that do not.[101] The primary difference appears to be that pre-light cultures have more broken-up sleep patterns.[101] For example, people without artificial light might go to sleep far sooner after the sun sets, but then wake up several times throughout the night, punctuating their sleep with periods of wakefulness, perhaps lasting several hours.[101]

The boundaries between sleeping and waking are blurred in these societies.[101] Some observers believe that nighttime sleep in these societies is most often split into two main periods, the first characterized primarily by deep sleep and the second by REM sleep.[101]

Some societies display a fragmented sleep pattern in which people sleep at all times of the day and night for shorter periods. In many nomadic or hunter-gatherer societies, people will sleep on and off throughout the day or night depending on what is happening.[101] Plentiful artificial light has been available in the industrialized West since at least the mid-19th century, and sleep patterns have changed significantly everywhere that lighting has been introduced.[101] In general, people sleep in a more concentrated burst through the night, going to sleep much later, although this is not always true.[101]

Historian Roger Ekirch thinks that the traditional pattern of "segmented sleep," as it is called, began to disappear among the urban upper class in Europe in the late 17th century and the change spread over the next 200 years; by the 1920s "the idea of a first and second sleep had receded entirely from our social consciousness."[103][104] Ekirch attributes the change to increases in "street lighting, domestic lighting and a surge in coffee houses," which slowly made nighttime a legitimate time for activity, decreasing the time available for rest.[104] Today in most societies people sleep during the night, but in very hot climates they may sleep during the day.[105] During Ramadan, many Muslims sleep during the day rather than at night.[106]

In some societies, people sleep with at least one other person (sometimes many) or with animals. In other cultures, people rarely sleep with anyone except for an intimate partner. In almost all societies, sleeping partners are strongly regulated by social standards. For example, a person might only sleep with the immediate family, the extended family, a spouse or romantic partner, children, children of a certain age, children of specific gender, peers of a certain gender, friends, peers of equal social rank, or with no one at all. Sleep may be an actively social time, depending on the sleep groupings, with no constraints on noise or activity.[101]

People sleep in a variety of locations. Some sleep directly on the ground; others on a skin or blanket; others sleep on platforms or beds. Some sleep with blankets, some with pillows, some with simple headrests, some with no head support. These choices are shaped by a variety of factors, such as climate, protection from predators, housing type, technology, personal preference, and the incidence of pests.[101]

Art[edit]

See also[edit]

Positions, practices, and rituals[edit]

References[edit]

  1. ^ a b c "Brain Basics: Understanding Sleep". Office of Communications and Public Liaison, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, US National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD. 2017. 
  2. ^ Randall, David K. (19 September 2012). "Book excerpt: How the lightbulb disrupted our sleeping patterns and changed the world". National Post. Retrieved 31 August 2016. "... the sudden introduction of bright nights during hours when it should be dark threw a wrench into a finely choreographed system of life. 
  3. ^ Bingham, Roger; Sejnowski, Terrence; Siegel, Jerry; Dyken, Mark Eric; Czeisler, Charles (February 2007). "Waking Up To Sleep" (Several conference videos). The Science Network. Retrieved 25 January 2008. 
  4. ^ Lulu Xie, Hongyi Kang1, Qiwu Xu, Michael J. Chen, Yonghong Liao, Meenakshisundaram Thiyagarajan, John O'Donne, Daniel J. Christensen, Charles Nicholson, Jeffrey J. Iliff, Takahiro Takano, Rashid Deane, Maiken Nedergaard (2013). "Sleep Drives Metabolite Clearance from the Adult Brain". Science. 342 (6156): 373–377. Bibcode:2013Sci...342..373X. doi:10.1126/science.1241224. PMC 3880190Freely accessible. PMID 24136970. Retrieved 18 October 2013. 
  5. ^ Parmeggiani (2011), Systemic Homeostasis and Poikilostasis in Sleep, p. 9–11.
  6. ^ Silber MH, Ancoli-Israel S, Bonnet MH, Chokroverty S, Grigg-Damberger MM, Hirshkowitz M, Kapen S, Keenan SA, Kryger MH, Penzel T, Pressman MR, Iber C (March 2007). "The visual scoring of sleep in adults" (PDF). Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine. 3 (2): 121–31. PMID 17557422. 
  7. ^ Brown, p. 1087.
  8. ^ Siegel Jerome M (2008). "Do all animals sleep?". Trends in Neurosciences. 31 (4): 208–13. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2008.02.001. PMID 18328577. 
  9. ^ a b c d e Åkerstedt Torbjorn; Billiard Michel; Bonnet Michael; Ficca Gianluca; Garma Lucile; Mariotti Maurizio; Salzarulo Piero; Schulz Hartmut (2002). "Awakening from Sleep". Sleep Medicine Reviews. 6 (4): 267–286. doi:10.1053/smrv.2001.0202. 
  10. ^ a b c d e f Roenneberga Till; Kuehnlea Tim; Judaa Myriam; Kantermanna Thomas; Allebrandta Karla; Gordijnb Marijke; Merrow Martha (2007). "Epidemiology of the human circadian clock". Sleep Medicine Reviews. 11 (6): 429–438. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2007.07.005. PMID 17936039. 
  11. ^ a b c d Waterhouse Jim; Fukuda Yumi; Morita Takeshi (2012). "Daily rhythms of the sleep-wake cycle". Journal of Physiological Anthropology. 31 (5). 
  12. ^ Basner Mathias; Dinges David F (2009). "Dubious Bargain: Trading Sleep for Leno and Letterman". Sleep. 32: 6. 
  13. ^ Tassi, P; Muzet, A (2000). "Sleep inertia". National Center for Biotechnology Information, U.S. National Library of Medicine. 4 (4): 341–353. doi:10.1053/smrv.2000.0098. PMID 12531174. 
  14. ^ Fenton, Reuven (29 August 2007). "Bio-alarm clocks set for perfect wake-up". Reuters. Retrieved 9 June 2008. 
  15. ^ Fuller Patrick M.; Gooley Joshua J.; Saper Clifford B. (2006). "Neurobiology of the Sleep-Wake Cycle: Sleep Architecture, Circadian Regulation, and Regulatory Feedback". Journal of Biological Rhythms. 21: 6. 
  16. ^ Phyllis C. Zee & Fred W. Turek (1999), "Introduction to Sleep and Circadian Rhythms", in Turek & Zee (eds.), Regulation of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms, pp. 1–17.
  17. ^ a b c d e f g Derk-Jan Dijk & Dale M. Edgar (1999), "Circadian and Homeostatic Control of Wakefulness and Sleep", in Turek & Zee (eds.), Regulation of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms', pp. 111–147'
  18. ^ Squire, Larry (2013). Fundamental neuroscience (4th ed.). Amsterdam: Elsevier/Academic Press. pp. 931–948. ISBN 9780123858702. 
  19. ^ a b Charles A. Czeisler & Kenneth P. Wright, Jr. (1999), "Influence of Light on Circadian Rhythmicity in Humans", in Turek & Zee (eds.), Regulation of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms, pp. 149–180.
  20. ^ a b Piotr Zlomanczuk & William J. Schwartz (1999). "Cellular and Molecular Mechanisms of Circadian Rhythms in Mammals", in Turek & Zee (eds.), Regulation of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms, pp. 309–342.
  21. ^ a b Eve Van Cauter & Karine Spiegel (1999). "Circadian and Sleep Control of Hormonal Secretions", in Turek & Zee (eds.), Regulation of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms, pp. 397–425.
  22. ^ a b Thomas A. Wehr (1999). "The Impact of Changes in Nightlength (Scotoperiod) on Human Sleep", in Turek & Zee (eds.), Regulation of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms, pp. 263–285.
  23. ^ Wyatt JK, Ritz-De Cecco A, Czeisler CA, Dijk DJ (1 October 1999). "Circadian temperature and melatonin rhythms, sleep, and neurobehavioral function in humans living on a 20-h day". Am J Physiol. 277 (4): R1152–R1163. PMID 10516257. 
  24. ^ Dagan Y (2002). "Circadian rhythm sleep disorders (CRSD)" (PDF: full text). Sleep Med Rev. 6 (1): 45–54. doi:10.1053/smrv.2001.0190. PMID 12531141. Retrieved 2016-06-05. Early onset of CRSD, the ease of diagnosis, the high frequency of misdiagnosis and erroneous treatment, the potentially harmful psychological and adjustment consequences, and the availability of promising treatments, all indicate the importance of greater awareness of these disorders. 
  25. ^ Naska A, Oikonomou E, Trichopoulou A, Psaltopoulou T, Trichopoulos D (2007). "Siesta in healthy adults and coronary mortality in the general population". Archives of Internal Medicine. 167 (3): 296–301. doi:10.1001/archinte.167.3.296. PMID 17296887. 
  26. ^ Tanaka, H; Tamura, N (2015). "Sleep education with self-help treatment and sleep health promotion for mental and physical wellness in Japan". Sleep and Biological Rhythms. 14: 89–99. doi:10.1007/s41105-015-0018-6. PMC 4732678Freely accessible. PMID 26855610. 
  27. ^ Barnes, C. M.; Lucianetti, L.; Bhave, D. P.; Christian, M. S. (2015). "You wouldn't like me when I'm sleepy: Leaders' sleep, daily abusive supervision, and work unit engagement". Academy of Management Journal. 58 (5): 1419–1437. doi:10.5465/amj.2013.1063. 
  28. ^ Harvey, A. G.; Payne, S. (2002). "The management of unwanted pre-sleep thoughts in insomnia: Distraction with imagery versus general distraction". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 40 (3): 267–277. doi:10.1016/s0005-7967(01)00012-2. PMID 11863237. 
  29. ^ He Y, Jones CR, Fujiki N, Xu Y, Guo B, Holder JL, Rossner MJ, Nishino S, Fu YH (2009). "The Transcriptional Repressor DEC2 Regulates Sleep Length in Mammals". Science. 325 (5942): 866–70. Bibcode:2009Sci...325..866H. doi:10.1126/science.1174443. PMC 2884988Freely accessible. PMID 19679812. 
  30. ^ Brown, pp. 1138–1102.
  31. ^ "The ABCC9 of Sleep: A Genetic Factor Regulates How Long We Sleep". Science Daily. Retrieved 21 August 2012. 
  32. ^ "Gene Cuts Need for Sleep - Sleep Disorders Including, Sleep Apnea, Narcolepsy, Insomnia, Snoring and Nightmares on MedicineNet.com". Archived from the original on 14 July 2011. Retrieved 11 June 2010. 
  33. ^ He Y, Jones CR, Fujiki N, Xu Y, Guo B, Holder JL, Rossner MJ, Nishino S, Fu YH (2009). "The transcriptional repressor DEC2 regulates sleep length in mammals". Science. 325 (5942): 866–70. Bibcode:2009Sci...325..866H. doi:10.1126/science.1174443. PMC 2884988Freely accessible. PMID 19679812. 
  34. ^ a b Jones, Samuel E.; Tyrrell, Jessica; Wood, Andrew R.; Beaumont, Robin N.; Ruth, Katherine S.; Tuke, Marcus A.; Yaghootkar, Hanieh; Hu, Youna; Teder-Laving, Maris (2016-08-01). "Genome-Wide Association Analyses in 128,266 Individuals Identifies New Morningness and Sleep Duration Loci". PLoS genetics. 12 (8): e1006125. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1006125. ISSN 1553-7404. PMID 27494321.  CC-BY icon.svg This content is available under the Creative Commons Attribution License 4.0.
  35. ^ Schwartz, Jonathan R.L; Roth, Thomas (24 January 2017). "Neurophysiology of Sleep and Wakefulness: Basic Science and Clinical Implications". Current Neuropharmacology. 6 (4): 367–378. doi:10.2174/157015908787386050. ISSN 1570-159X. PMC 2701283Freely accessible. PMID 19587857. 
  36. ^ Brown, pp. 1134–1138.
  37. ^ Gottselig JM, Adam M, Rétey JV, Khatami R, Achermann P, Landolt HP (March 2006). "Random number generation during sleep deprivation: effects of caffeine on response maintenance and stereotypy". Journal of Sleep Research. 15 (1): 31–40. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00497.x. PMID 16490000. 
  38. ^ Iglowstein I, Jenni OG, Molinari L, Largo RH (February 2003). "Sleep duration from infancy to adolescence: reference values and generational trends". Pediatrics. 111 (2): 302–7. doi:10.1542/peds.111.2.302. PMID 12563055. Thus, the shift in the evening bedtime across cohorts accounted for the substantial decrease in sleep duration in younger children between the 1970s and the 1990s... [A] more liberal parental attitude toward evening bedtime in the past decades is most likely responsible for the bedtime shift and for the decline of sleep duration... 
  39. ^ Huang, Z. L.; Zhang, Z; Qu, W. M. (2014). "Roles of adenosine and its receptors in sleep-wake regulation". International review of neurobiology. International Review of Neurobiology. 119: 349–71. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-801022-8.00014-3. ISBN 9780128010228. PMID 25175972.  |chapter= ignored (help)
  40. ^ "The brain from top to bottom: Molecules that build up and make you sleep". McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada. 
  41. ^ Clark, I; Landolt, H. P. (2017). "Coffee, caffeine, and sleep: A systematic review of epidemiological studies and randomized controlled trials". Sleep Medicine Reviews. 31: 70–78. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2016.01.006. PMID 26899133. 
  42. ^ Zisapel N (2007). "Sleep and sleep disturbances: biological basis and clinical implications". Cell Mol Life Sci. 64 (10): 1174–86. doi:10.1007/s00018-007-6529-9. PMID 17364142. 
  43. ^ a b de Benedictis, Tina; Larson, Heather; Kemp, Gina; Barston, Suzanne; Segal, Robert (2007). "Understanding Sleep: Sleep Needs, Cycles, and Stages". Helpguide.org. Archived from the original on 24 January 2008. Retrieved 25 January 2008. 
  44. ^ Reference list is found on image page in Commons: Commons:File:Effects of sleep deprivation.svg#References
  45. ^ Dijk DJ, Lockley SW (February 2002). "Functional Genomics of Sleep and Circadian Rhythm Invited Review: Integration of human sleep-wake regulation and circadian rhythmicity". J Appl Physiol. 92 (2): 852–62. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.00924.2001. PMID 11796701. Consolidation of sleep for 8 h or more is only observed when sleep is initiated ~6–8 h before the temperature nadir. 
  46. ^ Wyatt JK, Ritz-De Cecco A, Czeisler CA, Dijk DJ (1 October 1999). "Circadian temperature and melatonin rhythms, sleep, and neurobehavioral function in humans living on a 20-h day". Am J Physiol. 277 (4): R1152–R1163. PMID 10516257. ... significant homeostatic and circadian modulation of sleep structure, with the highest sleep efficiency occurring in sleep episodes bracketing the melatonin maximum and core body temperature minimum 
  47. ^ Lauderdale DS, Knutson KL, Yan LL, Liu K, Rathouz PJ (2008). "Self-Reported and Measured Sleep Duration: How Similar Are They?". Epidemiology. 19 (6): 838–45. doi:10.1097/EDE.0b013e318187a7b0. PMC 2785092Freely accessible. PMID 18854708. 
  48. ^ Insomnia Causes. Healthcommunities.com. Original Publication: 1 December 2000, Updated: 1 December 2007.
  49. ^ Rhonda Rowland (15 February 2002). "Experts challenge study linking sleep, life span". CNN. Retrieved 29 October 2013. 
  50. ^ Patel SR, Ayas NT, Malhotra MR, White DP, Schernhammer ES, Speizer FE, Stampfer MJ, Hu FB (May 2004). "A prospective study of sleep duration and mortality risk in women". Sleep. 27 (3): 440–4. PMID 15164896. 
  51. ^ Patel SR, Malhotra A, Gottlieb DJ, White DP, Hu FB (July 2006). "Correlates of long sleep duration". Sleep. 29 (7): 881–9. PMC 3500381Freely accessible. PMID 16895254. ; cf. Irwin MR, Ziegler M (February 2005). "Sleep deprivation potentiates activation of cardiovascular and catecholamine responses in abstinent alcoholics". Hypertension. 45 (2): 252–7. doi:10.1161/01.HYP.0000153517.44295.07. PMID 15642774. 
  52. ^ Ferrie JE, Shipley MJ, Cappuccio FP, Brunner E, Miller MA, Kumari M, Marmot MG (December 2007). "A prospective study of change in sleep duration: associations with mortality in the Whitehall II cohort". Sleep. 30 (12): 1659–66. PMC 2276139Freely accessible. PMID 18246975. Lay summaryUniversity of Warwick. 
  53. ^ Cappuccio, Francesco P.; Taggart, Frances M.; Kandala, Ngianga-Bakwin; Currie, Andrew; Peile, Ed; Stranges, Saverio; Miller, Michelle A. (2008-05-01). "Meta-analysis of short sleep duration and obesity in children and adults". Sleep. 31 (5): 619–626. ISSN 0161-8105. PMC 2398753Freely accessible. PMID 18517032. 
  54. ^ Schmid, Sebastian M.; Hallschmid, Manfred; Schultes, Bernd (2015-01-01). "The metabolic burden of sleep loss". The Lancet. Diabetes & Endocrinology. 3 (1): 52–62. doi:10.1016/S2213-8587(14)70012-9. ISSN 2213-8595. PMID 24731536. 
  55. ^ Thase ME (2006). "Depression and sleep: pathophysiology and treatment" (Free full text). Dialogues in clinical neuroscience. 8 (2): 217–226. ISSN 1294-8322. PMC 3181772Freely accessible. PMID 16889107. 
  56. ^ Mann, Joseph John; David J. Kupfer (1993). Biology of Depressive Disorders: Subtypes of depression and comorbid disorders, Part 2 (Google books). Springer. p. 49. ISBN 0-306-44296-5. Retrieved 24 July 2009. 
  57. ^ Dahl RE (2009). "The regulation of sleep and arousal: Development and psychopathology". Development and Psychopathology. 8 (1): 3–27. doi:10.1017/S0954579400006945. 
  58. ^ Jenni OG, Dahl RE (2008). "Sleep, cognition, and neuron, and emotion: A developmental review.". In Nelson CA, Luciana M. Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed.). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. pp. 807–817. ISBN 0262141043. 
  59. ^ a b Scher A (2005). "Infant sleep at 10 months of age as a window to cognitive development". Early Human Development. 81 (3): 289–92. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.07.005. PMID 15814211. 
  60. ^ Spruyt K, Aitken RJ, So K, Charlton M, Adamson TM, Horne RS (2008). "Relationship between sleep/wake patterns, temperament and overall development in term infants over the first year of life". Early Human Development. 84 (5): 289–96. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.07.002. PMID 17707119. 
  61. ^ a b Bernier A, Carlson SM, Bordeleau S, Carrier J (2010). "Relations between physiological and cognitive regulatory systems: infant sleep regulation and subsequent executive functioning". Child Development. 81 (6): 1739–52. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01507.x. PMID 21077861. 
  62. ^ Hupbach A, Gomez RL, Bootzin RR, Nadel L (2009). "Nap-dependent learning in infants". Developmental Science. 12 (6): 1007–12. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00837.x. PMID 19840054. 
  63. ^ a b c d e f g h i Hirshkowitz, Max; Whiton, Kaitlyn; et al. (14 January 2015). "National Sleep Foundation's sleep time duration recommendations: methodology and results summary". Sleep Health: Journal of the National Sleep Foundation. Elsevier Inc. 1: 40–43. doi:10.1016/j.sleh.2014.12.010. Retrieved 4 February 2015. 
  64. ^ "Backgrounder: Later School Start Times". National Sleep Foundation. n.d. Retrieved 2 October 2009. Teens are among those least likely to get enough sleep; while they need on average 914 hours of sleep per night... 
  65. ^ "How Much Sleep Is Enough?". National Heart, Lung and Blood Institute. Retrieved 25 July 2015. 
  66. ^ Marks GA, Shaffery JP, Oksenberg A, Speciale SG, Roffwarg HP (1995). "A functional role for REM sleep in brain maturation". Behavioural Brain Research. 69 (1–2): 1–11. doi:10.1016/0166-4328(95)00018-o. PMID 7546299. 
  67. ^ Mirmiran M, Scholtens J, van de Poll NE, Uylings HB, van der Gugten J, Boer GJ (April 1983). "Effects of experimental suppression of active (REM) sleep during early development upon adult brain and behavior in the rat". Brain Research. 283 (2–3): 277–86. doi:10.1016/0165-3806(83)90184-0. PMID 6850353. 
  68. ^ Morrissey MJ, Duntley SP, Anch AM, Nonneman R (2004). "Active sleep and its role in the prevention of apoptosis in the developing brain". Medical Hypotheses. 62 (6): 876–9. doi:10.1016/j.mehy.2004.01.014. PMID 15142640. 
  69. ^ Amanda Schaffer (27 May 2007). "Why do we Sleep?". Slate.com. Retrieved 23 August 2008. 
  70. ^ Turner TH, Drummond SP, Salamat JS, Brown GG (2007). "Effects of 42 hr sleep deprivation on component processes of verbal working memory". Neuropsychology. 21 (6): 787–795. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.21.6.787. PMID 17983292. 
  71. ^ Daltrozzo J, Claude L, Tillmann B, Bastuji H, Perrin F (2012). Zang, Yu-Feng, ed. "Working Memory Is Partially Preserved during Sleep". PLoS ONE. 7 (12): e50997. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...750997D. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0050997. PMC 3517624Freely accessible. PMID 23236418. 
  72. ^ Cited in Born J, Rasch B, Gais S (2006). "Sleep to remember". Neuroscientist. 12 (5): 410–24. doi:10.1177/1073858406292647. PMID 16957003. 
  73. ^ Marshall et al., 2006, as cited in Walker MP (2009). "The role of sleep in cognition and emotion". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1156: 168–97. Bibcode:2009NYASA1156..168W. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04416.x. PMID 19338508. 
  74. ^ Saper CB, Scammell TE, Lu J (2005). "Hypothalamic regulation of sleep and circadian rhythms". Nature. 437 (7063): 1257–63. Bibcode:2005Natur.437.1257S. doi:10.1038/nature04284. PMID 16251950. 
  75. ^ Stickgold R (2005). "Sleep-dependent memory consolidation". Nature. 437 (7063): 1272–8. Bibcode:2005Natur.437.1272S. doi:10.1038/nature04286. PMID 16251952. 
  76. ^ Capellini I, McNamara P, Preston BT, Nunn CL, Barton RA (2009). Sporns, Olaf, ed. "Does sleep play a role in memory consolidation? A comparative test". PLoS ONE. 4 (2): 4609. Bibcode:2009PLoSO...4.4609C. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0004609. PMC 2643482Freely accessible. PMID 19240803. 
  77. ^ Cirelli C, Gutierrez CM, Tononi G (2004). "Extensive and divergent effects of sleep and wakefulness on brain gene expression". Neuron. 41 (1): 35–43. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00814-6. PMID 14715133. 
  78. ^ J. Alan Hobson, Edward F. Pace-Scott, & Robert Stickgold (2000), "Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states", Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23.
  79. ^ Naiman, Rubin (2007). "How To Interpret Your Dreams". Allure. 17 (5): n/a. 
  80. ^ See Freud: The Interpretation of Dreams.
  81. ^ Connor, Steve (3 April 2009). "Revealed: why we need a good night's sleep". The Independent. Archived from the original on 2009-04-05. Retrieved 2 December 2010. 
  82. ^ Pinel, John P. J. (2011). Biopsychology, 8th Edition. Pearson Education, Inc. p. 359. ISBN 978-0-205-83256-9. 
  83. ^ Saladin, Kenneth S. (2012). Anatomy and Physiology: The Unity of Form and Function, 6th Edition. McGraw-Hill. p. 537. ISBN 978-0-07-337825-1. 
  84. ^ Hobson J. Alan; McCarley Robert W. (1977). "The Brain as a Dream-State Generator: An Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis of the Dream Process". American Journal of Psychiatry. 134 (12): 1335–1348. doi:10.1176/ajp.134.12.1335. PMID 21570. 
  85. ^ Brown, pp. 1146–1147.
  86. ^ Buman, M.P. and King, A.C. (2010). "Exercise as a Treatment to Enhance Sleep". American Journal of Lifestyle Medicine. 4 (6): 500–514. doi:10.1177/1559827610375532. 
  87. ^ López HH, Bracha AS, Bracha HS (2002). "Evidence based complementary intervention for insomnia" (PDF). Hawaii Med J. 61 (9): 192, 213. PMID 12422383. 
  88. ^ "What is Sleep Apnoea? (Sleep Apnea)". britishsnoring.co.uk. 
  89. ^ http://sleepfoundation.org/sleep-disorders-problems/sleep-apnea>
  90. ^ Max, D. T. The Secrets of Sleep National Geographic Magazine, May 2010.
  91. ^ Dugdale, David, C. (22 May 2011). Sleepwalking. US National institutes of health.
  92. ^ How Aging Changes Sleep Patterns by Allison Aubrey. Morning Edition, 3 August 2009.
  93. ^ a b Lee-chiong, Teofilo (24 April 2008). Sleep Medicine: Essentials and Review. Oxford University Press, USA. p. 52. ISBN 0-19-530659-7. 
  94. ^ Alcohol and Sleep. Alcoholism.about.com (10 January 2011). Retrieved on 1 December 2011.
  95. ^ Fred W. Turek & Charles A. Czeisler (1999). "Role of Melatonin in the Regulation of Sleep", in Turek & Zee (eds.), Regulation of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms, pp. 181–195.
  96. ^ Marijuana, Sleep and Dreams. psychologytoday.com. Retrieved on 10 February 2012.
  97. ^ Abarca C, Albrecht U, Spanagel R (June 2002). "Cocaine sensitization and reward are under the influence of circadian genes and rhythm". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 99 (13): 9026–30. Bibcode:2002PNAS...99.9026A. doi:10.1073/pnas.142039099. PMC 124417Freely accessible. PMID 12084940. 
  98. ^ Primary hypersomnia: Diagnostic Features. mindsite.com
  99. ^ Lindseth, G; Lindseth, P; Thompson, M (2013). "Nutritional effects on sleep". Western Journal of Nursing Research. 35 (4): 497–513. doi:10.1177/0193945911416379. PMID 21816963. 
  100. ^ Peuhkuri Katri; Sihvola Nora; Korpela Riitta (2012). "Diet Promotes Sleep Duration and Quality". Nutrition Research. 32 (5): 309–19. doi:10.1016/j.nutres.2012.03.009. PMID 22652369. 
  101. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k Carol M. Worthman; Melissa K. Melby. "6. Toward a comparative developmental ecology of human sleep" (PDF). A comparative developmental ecology (PDF). Emory University. 
  102. ^ Slumber's Unexplored Landscape. Science News Online (25 September 1999). Retrieved on 1 December 2011.
  103. ^ Ekirch, A. Roger (2001). "Sleep we have lost: Pre-industrial slumber in the British Isles". American Historical Review. 106 (2): 343–385. doi:10.2307/2651611. JSTOR 2651611. PMID 18680884. 
  104. ^ a b Hegarty, Stephanie (22 February 2012). "The myth of the eight-hour sleep". BBC News. Retrieved 22 February 2012. 
  105. ^ Huntington, Ellsworth (1915) Civilization and Climate. Yale University Press. p. 126
  106. ^ Dilara Hafiz; Imran Hafiz; Yasmine Hafiz (2009). The American Muslim Teenager's Handbook. ISBN 978-1416986997. 

Sources[edit]

External links[edit]