In
phonetics, a
vowel is a
sound in spoken
language, such as
English ''ah!'' or ''oh!'' , pronounced with an open
vocal tract so that there is no build-up of air pressure at any point above the
glottis. This contrasts with
consonants, such as English ''sh!'' , where there is a constriction or closure at some point along the vocal tract. A vowel is also understood to be
syllabic: an equivalent open but non-syllabic sound is called a
semivowel.
In all languages, vowels form the nucleus or peak of syllables, whereas consonants form the onset and (in languages that have them) coda. However, some languages also allow other sounds to form the nucleus of a syllable, such as the syllabic ''l'' in the English word ''table'' (the stroke under the ''l'' indicates that it is syllabic; the dot separates syllables), or the ''r'' in Slovene ''vrt'' "garden".
There is a conflict between the phonetic definition of "vowel" (a sound produced with no constriction in the vocal tract) and the phonological definition (a sound that forms the peak of a syllable). The approximants [j] and [w] illustrate this conflict: both are produced without much of a constriction in the vocal tract (so phonetically they seem to be vowel-like), but they occur on the edge of syllables, such as at the beginning of the English words "yet" and "wet" (which suggests that phonologically they are consonants). The American linguist Kenneth Pike suggested the terms 'vocoid' for a phonetic vowel and "vowel" for a phonological vowel, so using this terminology, [j] and [w] are classified as vocoids but not vowels.
The word ''vowel'' comes from the Latin word ''vocalis'', meaning "speaking", because in most languages words and thus speech are not possible without vowels. In English, the word ''vowel'' is commonly used to mean both vowel sounds and the written symbols that represent them.
==Articulation==
The articulatory features that distinguish different vowel sounds are said to determine the vowel's ''quality''. Daniel Jones developed the cardinal vowel system to describe vowels in terms of the common features ''height'' (vertical dimension), ''backness'' (horizontal dimension) and ''roundedness'' (lip position). These three parameters are indicated in the schematic IPA vowel diagram on the right. There are however still more possible features of vowel quality, such as the velum position (nasality), type of vocal fold vibration (phonation), and tongue root position.
===Height===
Vowel height is named for the vertical position of the tongue relative to either the roof of the mouth or the aperture of the jaw. In high vowels, such as and , the tongue is positioned high in the mouth, whereas in low vowels, such as , the tongue is positioned low in the mouth. The IPA prefers the terms close vowel and open vowel, respectively, which describes the jaw as being relatively open or closed. However, vowel height is an acoustic rather than articulatory quality, and is defined today not in terms of tongue height, or jaw openness, but according to the relative frequency of the first formant (F1). The higher the F1 value, the lower (more open) the vowel; height is thus inversely correlated to F1.
The International Phonetic Alphabet identifies seven different vowel heights:
close vowel (high vowel)
near-close vowel
close-mid vowel
mid vowel
open-mid vowel
near-open vowel
open vowel (low vowel)
True mid vowels do not contrast with both close-mid and open-mid in any language, and the letters are typically used for either close-mid or mid vowels.
Although English contrasts all six contrasting heights in its vowels, these are interdependent with differences in backness, and many are parts of diphthongs. It appears that some varieties of German have five contrasting vowel heights independently of length or other parameters. The Bavarian dialect of Amstetten has thirteen long vowels, reported to distinguish four heights (close, close-mid, mid, and near-open) each among the front unrounded, front rounded, and back rounded vowels, plus an open central vowel: . Otherwise, the usual limit on the number of contrasting vowel heights is four.
The parameter of vowel height appears to be the primary feature of vowels cross-linguistically in that all languages use height contrastively. No other parameter, such as front-back or rounded-unrounded (see below), is used in all languages. Some languages have vertical vowel systems in which, at least at a phonemic level, only height is used to distinguish vowels.
Backness
Vowel backness is named for the position of the tongue during the articulation of a vowel relative to the back of the mouth. In front vowels, such as , the tongue is positioned forward in the mouth, whereas in back vowels, such as , the tongue is positioned towards the back of the mouth. However, vowels are defined as back or front not according to actual articulation, but according to the relative frequency of the second
formant (F2). The higher the F2 value, the fronter the vowel; backness is thus inversely correlated to F2.
The International Phonetic Alphabet identifies five different degrees of vowel backness:
front vowel
near-front vowel
central vowel
near-back vowel
back vowel
Although English has vowels at all five degrees of backness, there is no known language that distinguishes all five without additional differences in height or rounding.
Roundedness
Roundedness refers to whether the lips are rounded or not. In most languages, roundedness is a reinforcing feature of mid to high back vowels, and is not distinctive. Usually the higher a back vowel is, the more intense the rounding. However, some languages treat roundedness and backness separately, such as French and German (with front rounded vowels), most
Uralic languages (
Estonian has a rounding contrast for and front vowels),
Turkic languages (with an unrounded ),
Vietnamese and
Korean with back unrounded vowels.
Nonetheless, even in languages such as German and Vietnamese, there is usually some phonetic correlation between rounding and backness: front rounded vowels tend to be less front than front unrounded vowels, and back unrounded vowels tend to be less back than back rounded vowels. That is, the placement of unrounded vowels to the left of rounded vowels on the IPA vowel chart is reflective of their typical position.
Different kinds of labialization are also possible. In mid to high rounded back vowels the lips are generally protruded ("pursed") outward, a phenomenon known as ''exolabial rounding'' because the insides of the lips are visible, whereas in mid to high rounded front vowels the lips are generally "compressed", with the margins of the lips pulled in and drawn towards each other, a phenomenon known as ''endolabial rounding.'' However, not all languages follow this pattern. The Japanese , for example, is an endolabial (compressed) back vowel, and sounds quite different from an English exolabial . Swedish and Norwegian are the only two known languages where this feature is contrastive, having both endo- and exo-labial close front rounded vowels and close central rounded vowels, respectively. In many phonetic treatments, both are considered types of rounding, but some phoneticians do not believe that these are subsets of a single phenomenon of rounding, and prefer instead the three independent terms ''rounded'' (exolabial), ''compressed'' (endolabial), and ''spread'' (unrounded).
Nasalization
Nasalization refers to whether some of the air escapes through the nose. In
nasal vowels, the
velum is lowered, and some air travels through the nasal cavity as well as the mouth. An oral vowel is a vowel in which all air escapes through the mouth.
French,
Polish and
Portuguese contrast nasal and oral vowels.
Phonation
Voicing describes whether the
vocal cords are vibrating during the articulation of a vowel. Most languages only have voiced vowels, but several Native American languages, such as
Cheyenne and
Totonac, contrast voiced and devoiced vowels. Vowels are devoiced in whispered speech. In
Japanese and
Quebec French, vowels that are between voiceless consonants are often devoiced.
Modal voice, creaky voice, and breathy voice (murmured vowels) are phonation types that are used contrastively in some languages. Often, these co-occur with tone or stress distinctions; in the Mon language, vowels pronounced in the high tone are also produced with creaky voice. In cases like this, it can be unclear whether it is the tone, the voicing type, or the pairing of the two that is being used for phonemic contrast. This combination of phonetic cues (i.e. phonation, tone, stress) is known as ''register'' or ''register complex''.
How a vowel is pronounced depends greatly on the type of phonation. For example, when speaking, [u] is pronounced with a raised tongue. However, this rule changes in sung diction. In this situation, vowels are split into two groups: Lip vowels (Ah, Oh, Oo, etc.) have no difference amongst them in terms of tongue (which is relaxed at the bottom of the mouth) or soft palate position (which is raised) when singing, whereas Tongue vowels (Eh, Ee) rely primarily on the position of the tongue.
Tongue root retraction
Advanced tongue root (ATR) is a feature common across much of Africa. The contrast between advanced and retracted tongue root resembles the tense/lax contrast acoustically, but they are articulated differently. ATR vowels involve noticeable tension in the vocal tract.
Secondary narrowings in the vocal tract
Pharyngealized vowels occur in some languages;
Sedang uses this contrast, as do the
Tungusic languages. Pharyngealisation is similar in articulation to retracted tongue root, but is
acoustically distinct.
A stronger degree of pharyngealisation occurs in the Northeast Caucasian languages and the Khoisan languages. These might be called epiglottalized, since the primary constriction is at the tip of the epiglottis.
The greatest degree of pharyngealisation is found in the strident vowels of the Khoisan languages, where the larynx is raised, and the pharynx constricted, so that either the epiglottis or the arytenoid cartilages vibrate instead of the vocal cords.
Note that the terms ''pharyngealized'', ''epiglottalized'', ''strident'', and ''sphincteric'' are sometimes used interchangeably.
Rhotic vowels
Rhotic vowels are the "R-colored vowels" of English and a few other languages.
Tenseness/checked vowels vs. free vowels
Tenseness is used to describe the opposition of ''tense vowels'' as in ''leap'', ''suit'' vs. ''lax vowels'' as in ''lip'', ''soot''. This opposition has traditionally been thought to be a result of greater muscular tension, though phonetic experiments have repeatedly failed to show this.
Unlike the other features of vowel quality, tenseness is only applicable to the few languages that have this opposition (mainly Germanic languages, e.g. English), whereas the vowels of the other languages (e.g. Spanish) cannot be described with respect to tenseness in any meaningful way. In discourse about the English language, "tense and lax" are often used interchangeably with "long and short", respectively, because the features are concomitant in the common varieties of English. This cannot be applied to all English dialects or other languages.
In most Germanic languages, lax vowels can only occur in closed syllables. Therefore, they are also known as checked vowels, whereas the tense vowels are called free vowels since they can occur in any kind of syllable.
Acoustics
:''Related article:
Phonetics''
The acoustics of vowels are fairly well understood. The different vowel qualities are realized in acoustic analyses of vowels by the relative values of the formants, acoustic resonances of the vocal tract which show up as dark bands on a spectrogram. The vocal tract acts as a resonant cavity, and the position of the jaw, lips, and tongue affect the parameters of the resonant cavity, resulting in different formant values. The acoustics of vowels can be visualized using spectrograms, which display the acoustic energy at each frequency, and how this changes with time.
The first formant, abbreviated "F1", corresponds to vowel openness (vowel height). Open vowels have high F1 frequencies while close vowels have low F1 frequencies, as can be seen at right: The and have similar low first formants, whereas has a higher formant.
The second formant, F2, corresponds to vowel frontness. Back vowels have low F2 frequencies while front vowels have high F2 frequencies. This is very clear at right, where the front vowel has a much higher F2 frequency than the other two vowels. However, in open vowels the high F1 frequency forces a rise in the F2 frequency as well, so an alternative measure of frontness is the ''difference'' between the first and second formants. For this reason, some people prefer to plot as F1 vs. F2 – F1. (This dimension is usually called 'backness' rather than 'frontness', but the term 'backness' can be counterintuitive when discussing formants.)
In the third edition of his textbook, Peter Ladefoged recommended use of plots of F1 against F2 – F1 to represent vowel quality. However, in the fourth edition, he changed to adopt a simple plot of F1 against F2, and this simple plot of F1 against F2 was maintained for the fifth (and final) edition of the book. Katrina Hayward compares the two types of plots and concludes that plotting of F1 against F2 – F1 "is not very satisfactory because of its effect on the placing of the central vowels", so she also recommends use of a simple plot of F1 against F2. In fact, this kind of plot of F1 against F2 has been used by analysts to show the quality of the vowels in a wide range of languages, including RP British English, the Queen's English, American English, Singapore English, Brunei English, North Frisian, Turkish Kabardian, and various indigenous Australian languages.
R-colored vowels are characterized by lowered F3 values.
Rounding is generally realized by a complex relationship between F2 and F3 that tends to reinforce vowel backness. One effect of this is that back vowels are most commonly rounded while front vowels are most commonly unrounded; another is that rounded vowels tend to plot to the right of unrounded vowels in vowel charts. That is, there is a reason for plotting vowel pairs the way they are.
Prosody and intonation
The features of vowel
prosody are often described independently from vowel quality. In non-linear phonetics, they are located on parallel layers. The features of vowel prosody are usually considered not to apply to the vowel itself, but to the
syllable, as some languages do not contrast vowel length separately from syllable length.
Intonation encompasses the changes in pitch, intensity, and speed of an utterance over time. In tonal languages, in most cases the tone of a syllable is carried by the vowel, meaning that the relative pitch or the pitch contour that marks the tone is superimposed on the vowel. If a syllable has a high tone, for example, the pitch of the vowel will be high. If the syllable has a falling tone, then the pitch of the vowel will fall from high to low over the course of uttering the vowel.
Length or quantity refers to the abstracted duration of the vowel. In some analyses this feature is described as a feature of the vowel quality, not of the prosody. Japanese, Finnish, Hungarian, Arabic and Latin have a two-way phonemic contrast between short and long vowels. The Mixe language has a three-way contrast among short, half-long, and long vowels, and this has been reported for a few other languages, though not always as a phonemic distinction. Long vowels are written in the IPA with a triangular colon, which has two equilateral triangles pointing at each other in place of dots (). The IPA symbol for half-long vowels is the top half of this (). Longer vowels are sometimes claimed, but these are always divided between two syllables.
The length of the vowel is a grammatical abstraction, and there may be more phonologically distinctive lengths. For example, in Finnish, there are five different physical lengths, because stress is marked with length on both grammatically long and short vowels. However, Finnish stress is not lexical and is always on the first two moras, thus this variation serves to separate words from each other.
In non-tonal languages, like English, intonation encompasses lexical stress. A stressed syllable will typically be pronounced with a higher pitch, intensity, and length than unstressed syllables. For example in the word ''intensity'', the vowel represented by the letter 'e' is stressed, so it is longer and pronounced with a higher pitch and intensity than the other vowels.
Monophthongs, diphthongs, triphthongs
A vowel sound whose quality doesn't change over the duration of the vowel is called a
monophthong. Monophthongs are sometimes called "pure" or "stable" vowels. A vowel sound that glides from one quality to another is called a
diphthong, and a vowel sound that glides successively through three qualities is a
triphthong.
All languages have monophthongs and many languages have diphthongs, but triphthongs or vowel sounds with even more target qualities are relatively rare cross-linguistically. English has all three types: the vowel sound in ''hit'' is a monophthong , the vowel sound in ''boy'' is in most dialects a diphthong , and the vowel sounds of ''flower,'' , form a triphthong or disyllable, depending on dialect.
In phonology, diphthongs and triphthongs are distinguished from sequences of monophthongs by whether the vowel sound may be analyzed into different phonemes or not. For example, the vowel sounds in a two-syllable pronunciation of the word ''flower'' () phonetically form a disyllabic triphthong, but are phonologically a sequence of a diphthong (represented by the letters ) and a monophthong (represented by the letters ). Some linguists use the terms ''diphthong'' and ''triphthong'' only in this phonemic sense.
Written vowels
The name "vowel" is often used for the symbols that represent vowel sounds in a language's
writing system, particularly if the language uses an
alphabet. In writing systems based on the
Latin alphabet, the letters ''A'', ''E'', ''I'', ''O'', ''U'', and sometimes ''Y'' are all used to represent vowels. However, not all of these letters represent vowels in all languages, or even consistently within one language (some of them, especially ''W'' and ''Y'', are also used to represent
approximants). Moreover, a vowel might be represented by a letter usually reserved for consonants, or a combination of letters, particularly where one letter represents several sounds at once, or vice versa; examples from English include ''igh'' in "thigh" and ''x'' in "x-ray". In addition, extensions of the Latin alphabet have such independent vowel letters as ''Ä'', ''Ö'', ''Ü'', ''Å'', ''Æ'', and ''Ø''.
The phonetic values vary considerably by language, and some languages use ''I'' and ''Y'' for the consonant , e.g., initial ''I'' in Romanian and initial ''Y'' in English. In the original Latin alphabet, there was no written distinction between ''V'' and ''U'', and the letter represented the approximant and the vowels and . In Modern Welsh, the letter ''W'' represents these same sounds. Similarly, in Creek, the letter ''V'' stands for . There is not necessarily a direct one-to-one correspondence between the vowel sounds of a language and the vowel letters. Many languages that use a form of the Latin alphabet have more vowel sounds than can be represented by the standard set of five vowel letters. In English spelling, the five letters ''A'' ''E'' ''I'' ''O'' and ''U'' can represent a variety of vowel sounds, while the letter ''Y'' frequently represents vowels (as in e.g., "gym" or "happy") or diphthongs (e.g. "cry", "thyme"); ''W'' is used in representing some diphthongs (as in "cow") and to represent a monophthong in the borrowed words "" and "".
Other languages cope with the limitation in the number of Latin vowel letters in similar ways. Many languages make extensive use of combinations of letters to represent various sounds. Other languages use vowel letters with modifications, e.g., ''Ä'' in Finnish, or add diacritical marks, like umlauts, to vowels to represent the variety of possible vowel sounds. Some languages have also constructed additional vowel letters by modifying the standard Latin vowels in other ways, such as ''æ'' or ''ø'' that are found in some of the Scandinavian languages. The International Phonetic Alphabet has a set of 28 symbols to represent the range of basic vowel qualities, and a further set of diacritics to denote variations from the basic vowel.
Vowel shifts
The differences in pronunciation of vowel letters between English and other languages can be accounted for by the
Great Vowel Shift. After printing was introduced to England, and therefore after spelling was more or less standardized, a series of dramatic changes in the pronunciation of the vowel phonemes did occur, and continued into recent centuries, but were not reflected in the spelling system. This has led to numerous inconsistencies in the spelling of English vowel sounds and the pronunciation of English vowel letters (and to the mispronunciation of foreign words and names by speakers of English).
The existence of vowel shifts should serve as a caution flag to anyone who is trying to pronounce an ancient language or, indeed, any poetry (in any language) from two centuries ago or earlier.
Audio samples
See also
Zero consonant
Mater lectionis
Inherent vowel
Use of vowels in languages
The importance of vowels in distinguishing one word from another varies from language to language. The alphabets used to write the Semitic languages, such as the
Hebrew alphabet and the
Arabic alphabet, do not ordinarily mark all the vowels, since they are frequently unnecessary in identifying a word. These alphabets are technically called
abjads. Although it is possible to construct simple English sentences that can be understood without written vowels (
''cn y rd ths?''), extended passages of English lacking written vowels can be difficult to understand (consider ''dd'', which could be any of ''dad, dada, dado, dead, deed, did, died, diode, dodo, dud'', or ''dude'').
In most languages, vowels serve mainly to distinguish separate lexemes, rather than different inflectional forms of the same lexeme as they commonly do in the Semitic languages. For example, while English ''man'' becomes ''men'' in the plural, ''moon'' is not a different form of the same word.
Nearly all languages have at least three phonemic vowels, usually as in Classical Arabic and Inuktitut (or as in Quechua), though Adyghe and many Sepik languages have a vertical vowel system of . Very few languages have fewer, though some Arrernte, Circassian, Ndu languages have been argued to have just two, and , with being epenthetic.
The rarest vowels cataloged are ɜ (has just been cataloged in Paicî and Received Pronunciation English) and (Early Modern English and Russian).
It is not straightforward to say which language has the most vowels, since that depends on how they are counted. For example, long vowels, nasal vowels, and various phonations may or may not be counted separately; indeed, it may sometimes be unclear if phonation belongs to the vowels or the consonants of a language. If such things are ignored and only vowels with dedicated IPA letters ('vowel qualities') are considered, then very few languages have more than ten. The Germanic languages have some of the largest inventories: Standard Swedish has seventeen contrasting simple vowels, nine long and eight short (), while the Amstetten dialect of Bavarian has been reported to have thirteen long vowels: . The situation can be quite disparate within a same family language: Spanish and French are two closely related Romance languages but Spanish has only five vowels while classical French has sixteen of them: . The Mon–Khmer languages of Southeast Asia also have some large inventories, such as the eleven vowels of Vietnamese: . Wu has the largest inventories of Chinese; the Jinhui dialect of Wu (金汇方言Dônđäc as an alternate name)) has also been reported to have twenty vowels: nineteen normal vowels, , plus restricted . The Taa language, spoken mainly in Botswana and Namibia, is extremely unusual in having (depending on analysis) between 20 and 31 vowels.
One of the most common vowels is ; it is nearly universal for a language to have at least one open vowel, though most dialects of English have an and a —and often an , all open vowels—but no central . Some Tagalog- and Cebuano-speakers have rather than , and Dhangu Yolngu is described as having , without any peripheral vowels. is also extremely common, though Quileute has without any close vowels, at least as they are pronounced when long. The third vowel of Arabic-type three-vowel system, , is considerably less common. A large fraction of the languages of North America happen to have a four-vowel system without : ; Aztec is an example.
See also
English phonology
List of phonetics topics
Scale of vowels
Table of vowels
Vowel coalescence
Words without vowels
Words without consonants
Great Vowel Shift
References
Bibliography
''Handbook of the International Phonetic Association'', 1999. Cambridge University ISBN 978-0-521-63751-0
Johnson, Keith, ''Acoustic & Auditory Phonetics'', second edition, 2003. Blackwell ISBN 978-1-4051-0123-3
Korhonen, Mikko. ''Koltansaamen opas'', 1973. Castreanum ISBN 978-951-45-0189-0
Ladefoged, Peter, ''A Course in Phonetics'', fifth edition, 2006. Boston, MA: Thomson Wadsworth ISBN 978-1-4130-2079-3
Ladefoged, Peter, ''Elements of Acoustic Phonetics'', 1995. University of Chicago ISBN 978-0-226-46764-1
Ladefoged, Peter, ''Vowels and Consonants: An Introduction to the Sounds of Languages'', 2000. Blackwell ISBN 978-0-631-21412-0.
Stevens, Kenneth N. (1998). ''Acoustic phonetics''. Current studies in linguistics (No. 30). Cambridge, MA: MIT. ISBN 978-0-262-19404-4.
Watt, D. and Tillotson, J. (2001). A spectrographic analysis of vowel fronting in Bradford English. ''English World-Wide'' 22:2, 269–302. Available at http://www.abdn.ac.uk/langling/resources/Watt-Tillotson2001.pdf
External links
IPA chart with MP3 sound files
IPA vowel chart with AIFF sound files
Vowel charts for several different languages and dialects measuring F1 and F2
Materials for measuring and plotting vowel formants
Vowels and Consonants Online examples from Ladefoged's ''Vowels and Consonants'', referenced above.
Category:Vowels
Category:Phonetics
af:Klinker
als:Vokal
ar:حرف مصوت
roa-rup:Vocalu
ast:Vocal
be:Галосны гук
bo:དབྱངས་ཡིག
bs:Samoglasnik
br:Vogalenn
bg:Гласен звук
ca:Vocal
cv:Уçă сасăсем
cs:Samohláska
cy:Llafariad
da:Vokal (sprog)
de:Vokal
et:Täishäälik
el:Φωνήεν
es:Vocal
eo:Vokalo
ext:Vocal
eu:Bokal
fa:واکه
fr:Voyelle
fy:Fokaal
gd:Fuaimreag
gl:Vogal
ko:홀소리
hi:स्वर वर्ण
hr:Samoglasnik
io:Vokalo
id:Vokal (linguistik)
is:Sérhljóð
it:Vocale
he:תנועה (פונולוגיה)
jv:Vokal
ka:ხმოვანი
kk:Дауыстылар
kw:Bogalen
sw:Vokali
ht:Vwayèl
la:Vocalis
lv:Patskanis
lb:Vokal
li:Kleenker
ln:Moleli
hu:Magánhangzó
mk:Самогласка
ms:Vokal
nah:Tzilīnitl
nl:Klinker (klank)
ja:母音
no:Vokal
nn:Vokal
pl:Samogłoska
pt:Vogal
ro:Vocală
qu:Hanllalli
ru:Гласные
scn:Vocali
simple:Vowel
sk:Samohláska
sl:Samoglasnik
ckb:پیتی بزوێن
sr:Самогласник
sh:Samoglasnik
fi:Vokaali
sv:Vokal
ta:உயிரொலி
th:สระ (สัทศาสตร์)
tr:Ünlü harf
uk:Голосний звук
vi:Nguyên âm
fiu-vro:Vabahelü
wa:Voyale
war:Tiringgan
yi:וואקאל
zh-yue:元音
zh:元音