-
Benedict Carey
Benedict Carey (born 1960) is an American journalist and reporter on medical and science topics for The New York Times.
http://wn.com/Benedict_Carey -
Cosmos Rossellius
Cosmos Rossellius (died 1578) was a Florentine Dominican friar who wrote a book about memory.
http://wn.com/Cosmos_Rossellius -
George Sperling
George Sperling is a cognitive psychologist who documented the existence of iconic memory (one of the sensory memory subtypes). Sperling, through several experiments, was able to prove his hypothesis that human beings store a perfect image of the visual world for a brief moment, before it is discarded from memory.
http://wn.com/George_Sperling -
Herbert Simon
Herbert Alexander Simon (June 15, 1916 – February 9, 2001) was an American political scientist, sociologist, and psychologist, and professor—most notably at Carnegie Mellon University—whose research ranged across the fields of cognitive psychology, cognitive science, computer science, public administration, economics, management, philosophy of science, sociology, and political science. With almost a thousand very highly cited publications, he is one of the most influential social scientists of the 20th century.
http://wn.com/Herbert_Simon -
Richard C. Atkinson
Richard C. Atkinson (born 19 March 1929) is an American professor of psychology and academic administrator. He is the former president and regent of the University of California system, and former chancellor of U.C. San Diego.
http://wn.com/Richard_C_Atkinson -
Richard Shiffrin
Richard Shiffrin is the Luther Dana Waterman Professor of cognitive science for the Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences at Indiana University, Bloomington. Shiffrin has contributed a number of theories of attention and memory to the field of psychology. He co-authored the Atkinson-Shiffrin model of memory in 1968 with Richard Atkinson, who was his academic adviser at the time. In 1977, he published a theory of attention with Walter Schneider . With Jeroen G.W. Raaijmakers in 1980, Shiffrin published the Search of Associative Memory (SAM) model, which has served as the standard model of recall for cognitive psychologists well into the 2000s. He extended the SAM model with the Retrieving Effectively From Memory (REM) model in 1997 with Mark Steyvers.
http://wn.com/Richard_Shiffrin
-
Bell Laboratories (also known as Bell Labs and formerly known as AT&T; Bell Laboratories and Bell Telephone Laboratories) is the research and development organization of Alcatel-Lucent and previously of the American Telephone & Telegraph Company (AT&T;).
http://wn.com/Bell_Labs
- Alzheimer's Disease
- amnesia
- amygdala
- Anomia
- Apolipoprotein E
- Art of memory
- attention span
- basal ganglia
- Bell Labs
- Benedict Carey
- brain damage
- Cell (journal)
- cerebellum
- Cognition
- cognitive psychology
- conscious
- cortisol
- Cosmos Rossellius
- Cram school
- declarative learning
- declarative memory
- DNA methylation
- Déjà vu
- Emotion and memory
- Encoding (memory)
- epinephrine
- episodic memory
- explicit memory
- Flashbulb memory
- frontal lobe
- Fuzzy-trace theory
- George Sperling
- Georges Chapouthier
- healthy eating
- Herbert Simon
- hippocampus
- Hyperthymesia
- implicit
- implicit memory
- information
- Interference theory
- KIBRA
- knowledge
- Korsakoff's syndrome
- learning
- lobes of the brain
- long-term depression
- mammillary bodies
- Memory and aging
- memory disorder
- memory loss
- mental image
- Method of loci
- millisecond
- mnemonic
- motor learning
- motor skill
- Muscle memory
- Music-related memory
- neurology
- neuroscience
- New Scientist
- New York Times
- Nominal Aphasia
- Ohio State
- paradigm
- parietal lobe
- Parkinson's disease
- phenomenon
- philosophy
- physical fitness
- Piphilology
- prefrontal cortex
- priming (psychology)
- procedural memory
- prospective memory
- psychology
- Recollection
- retrospective memory
- Reuters
- Richard C. Atkinson
- Richard Shiffrin
- Rote learning
- semantic memory
- semantic similarity
- sleep
- spacing effect
- Storage (memory)
- Stress management
- striatum
- synapse
- Synaptic plasticity
- telephone number
- Tip of the tongue
- Unconscious mind
- University
- visual alphabets
- Visual memory
- Zeigarnik effect
Memoriance
Releases by year: 1976 |
Releases by album:
Et après |
Album releases
Et après
(Released 1976)
Your Memorial
Releases by album:
Seasons |
Album releases
Seasons
(Released 2008)
-
Spring
-
Learn Your Lies
-
Return to Reality
-
Summer
-
Turn It All Around
-
Vanquished
-
Autumn
-
Empryean
-
Seasons
-
Winter
-
Cease: The Eliminator
The Memories
Releases by album:
Album releases
The Memorials
Releases by album:
Album releases
The Memorials
(Released 2011)
-
We Go to War
-
Natural Disaster
-
Day Dreamer
-
Let's Party
-
Westcoast
-
Dream
-
Gtfomf
-
Real
-
Born to Shine
-
Enough
-
Why Me?
-
Give Me the Stuff
-
I Remember You
-
We Go to War (radio edit)
Album releases
Seasons (Released 2008)
- Spring
- Learn Your Lies
- Return to Reality
- Summer
- Turn It All Around
- Vanquished
- Autumn
- Empryean
- Seasons
- Winter
- Cease: The Eliminator
The Memories
Releases by album:
Album releases
The Memorials
Releases by album:
Album releases
The Memorials
(Released 2011)
-
We Go to War
-
Natural Disaster
-
Day Dreamer
-
Let's Party
-
Westcoast
-
Dream
-
Gtfomf
-
Real
-
Born to Shine
-
Enough
-
Why Me?
-
Give Me the Stuff
-
I Remember You
-
We Go to War (radio edit)
Album releases
The Memorials (Released 2011)
- We Go to War
- Natural Disaster
- Day Dreamer
- Let's Party
- Westcoast
- Dream
- Gtfomf
- Real
- Born to Shine
- Enough
- Why Me?
- Give Me the Stuff
- I Remember You
- We Go to War (radio edit)
- Order: Reorder
- Duration: 3:29
- Published: 08 Feb 2010
- Uploaded: 03 Dec 2011
- Author: davidguetta
- Order: Reorder
- Duration: 3:31
- Published: 16 Oct 2009
- Uploaded: 02 Dec 2011
- Author: schwarzgelbforever
- Order: Reorder
- Duration: 3:14
- Published: 14 Sep 2010
- Uploaded: 02 Dec 2011
- Author: EpitaphRecords
- Order: Reorder
- Duration: 3:53
- Published: 23 Jul 2007
- Uploaded: 02 Dec 2011
- Author: UrChatNoir
- Order: Reorder
- Duration: 3:49
- Published: 19 Oct 2007
- Uploaded: 02 Dec 2011
- Author: wtofficial
- Order: Reorder
- Duration: 3:48
- Published: 28 Aug 2009
- Uploaded: 02 Dec 2011
- Author: shark18741045
- Order: Reorder
- Duration: 4:02
- Published: 11 Jan 2009
- Uploaded: 02 Dec 2011
- Author: AnnaMayWongSociety
- Order: Reorder
- Duration: 3:50
- Published: 27 Oct 2009
- Uploaded: 02 Dec 2011
- Author: wtofficial
- Order: Reorder
- Duration: 3:02
- Published: 14 Sep 2010
- Uploaded: 02 Dec 2011
- Author: pampetersonality
- Order: Reorder
- Duration: 2:22
- Published: 23 Nov 2011
- Uploaded: 02 Dec 2011
- Author: KarminVEVO
- Order: Reorder
- Duration: 3:56
- Published: 10 Jun 2008
- Uploaded: 02 Dec 2011
- Author: eowynprincsold
- Order: Reorder
- Duration: 3:54
- Published: 12 Aug 2009
- Uploaded: 02 Dec 2011
- Author: d4rk3rst4r
- Order: Reorder
- Duration: 3:18
- Published: 11 Jul 2008
- Uploaded: 02 Dec 2011
- Author: patriklovesyou
- Order: Reorder
- Duration: 4:28
- Published: 28 Oct 2008
- Uploaded: 02 Dec 2011
- Author: RoadrunnerRecords
- Order: Reorder
- Duration: 5:09
- Published: 25 Nov 2010
- Uploaded: 02 Dec 2011
- Author: bulletvalentineVEVO
- Order: Reorder
- Duration: 3:44
- Published: 14 Sep 2009
- Uploaded: 02 Dec 2011
- Author: AlLyKaT303
- Order: Reorder
- Duration: 3:53
- Published: 09 Jun 2011
- Uploaded: 02 Dec 2011
- Author: CarparkRecords
size: 10.3Kb
size: 1.0Kb
- aging
- Alzheimer's Disease
- amnesia
- amygdala
- Anomia
- Apolipoprotein E
- Art of memory
- attention span
- basal ganglia
- Bell Labs
- Benedict Carey
- brain damage
- Cell (journal)
- cerebellum
- Cognition
- cognitive psychology
- conscious
- cortisol
- Cosmos Rossellius
- Cram school
- declarative learning
- declarative memory
- DNA methylation
- Déjà vu
- Emotion and memory
- Encoding (memory)
- epinephrine
- episodic memory
- explicit memory
- Flashbulb memory
- frontal lobe
- Fuzzy-trace theory
- George Sperling
- Georges Chapouthier
- healthy eating
- Herbert Simon
- hippocampus
- Hyperthymesia
- implicit
- implicit memory
- information
- Interference theory
- KIBRA
- knowledge
- Korsakoff's syndrome
- learning
- lobes of the brain
- long-term depression
- mammillary bodies
- Memory and aging
- memory disorder
- memory loss
- mental image
- Method of loci
- millisecond
- mnemonic
- motor learning
- motor skill
- Muscle memory
- Music-related memory
size: 0.9Kb
size: 2.1Kb
size: 1.4Kb
size: 7.3Kb
size: 5.0Kb
size: 10.0Kb
In psychology, memory is an organism's ability to store, retain, and recall information and experiences. Traditional studies of memory began in the fields of philosophy, including techniques of artificially enhancing memory. During the late nineteenth and early twentieth century, scientists have put memory within the paradigm of cognitive psychology. In recent decades, it has become one of the principal pillars of a branch of science called cognitive neuroscience, an interdisciplinary link between cognitive psychology and neuroscience.
Processes
From an information processing perspective there are three main stages in the formation and retrieval of memory:
Sensory memory
Sensory memory corresponds approximately to the initial 200–500 milliseconds after an item is perceived. The ability to look at an item, and remember what it looked like with just a second of observation, or memorisation, is an example of sensory memory. With very short presentations, participants often report that they seem to "see" more than they can actually report. The first experiments exploring this form of sensory memory were conducted by George Sperling (1960) using the "partial report paradigm". Subjects were presented with a grid of 12 letters, arranged into three rows of four. After a brief presentation, subjects were then played either a high, medium or low tone, cuing them which of the rows to report. Based on these partial report experiments, Sperling was able to show that the capacity of sensory memory was approximately 12 items, but that it degraded very quickly (within a few hundred milliseconds). Because this form of memory degrades so quickly, participants would see the display, but be unable to report all of the items (12 in the "whole report" procedure) before they decayed. This type of memory cannot be prolonged via rehearsal.
Short-term
Short-term memory allows recall for a period of several seconds to a minute without rehearsal. Its capacity is also very limited: George A. Miller (1956), when working at Bell Laboratories, conducted experiments showing that the store of short-term memory was 7±2 items (the title of his famous paper, "The magical number 7±2"). Modern estimates of the capacity of short-term memory are lower, typically on the order of 4–5 items, however, memory capacity can be increased through a process called chunking. For example, in recalling a ten-digit telephone number, a person could chunk the digits into three groups: first, the area code (such as 215), then a three-digit chunk (123) and lastly a four-digit chunk (4567). This method of remembering telephone numbers is far more effective than attempting to remember a string of 10 digits; this is because we are able to chunk the information into meaningful groups of numbers. Herbert Simon showed that the ideal size for chunking letters and numbers, meaningful or not, was three. This may be reflected in some countries in the tendency to remember telephone numbers as several chunks of three numbers with the final four-number groups, generally broken down into two groups of two.Short-term memory is believed to rely mostly on an acoustic code for storing information, and to a lesser extent a visual code. Conrad (1964) found that test subjects had more difficulty recalling collections of letters that were acoustically similar (e.g. E, P, D). Confusion with recalling acoustically similar letters rather than visually similar letters implies that the letters were encoded acoustically. Conrad's (1964) study however, deals with the encoding of written text, thus while memory of written language may rely on acoustic components, generalisations to all forms of memory cannot be made.
However, some individuals have been reported to be able to remember large amounts of information, quickly, and be able to recall that information in seconds.
Long-term
The storage in sensory memory and short-term memory generally have a strictly limited capacity and duration, which means that information is available only for a certain period of time, but is not retained indefinitely. By contrast, long-term memory can store much larger quantities of information for potentially unlimited duration (sometimes a whole life span). Its capacity is immeasurably large. For example, given a random seven-digit number we may remember it for only a few seconds before forgetting, suggesting it was stored in our short-term memory. On the other hand, we can remember telephone numbers for many years through repetition; this information is said to be stored in long-term memory.While short-term memory encodes information acoustically, long-term memory encodes it semantically: Baddeley (1966) discovered that after 20 minutes, test subjects had the most difficulty recalling a collection of words that had similar meanings (e.g. big, large, great, huge).
Short-term memory is supported by transient patterns of neuronal communication, dependent on regions of the frontal lobe (especially dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) and the parietal lobe. Long-term memories, on the other hand, are maintained by more stable and permanent changes in neural connections widely spread throughout the brain. The hippocampus is essential (for learning new information) to the consolidation of information from short-term to long-term memory, although it does not seem to store information itself. Without the hippocampus, new memories are unable to be stored into long-term memory, and there will be a very short attention span. Furthermore, it may be involved in changing neural connections for a period of three months or more after the initial learning. One of the primary functions of sleep is thought to be improving consolidation of information, as several studies have demonstrated that memory depends on getting sufficient sleep between training and test. Additionally, data obtained from neuroimaging studies have shown activation patterns in the sleeping brain which mirror those recorded during the learning of tasks from the previous day, suggesting that new memories may be solidified through such rehearsal.
Research has suggested that long-term memory storage in humans may be regulated by DNA methylation.
Models
Models of memory provide abstract representations of how memory is believed to work. Below are several models proposed over the years by various psychologists. Note that there is some controversy as to whether there are several memory structures, for example, Tarnow (2005) finds that it is likely that there is only one memory structure between 6 and 600 seconds.
Atkinson-Shiffrin model
The multi-store model (also known as Atkinson-Shiffrin memory model) was first recognised in 1968 by Atkinson and Shiffrin.The multi-store model has been criticised for being too simplistic. For instance, long-term memory is believed to be actually made up of multiple subcomponents, such as episodic and procedural memory. It also proposes that rehearsal is the only mechanism by which information eventually reaches long-term storage, but evidence shows us capable of remembering things without rehearsal.
The model also shows all the memory stores as being a single unit whereas research into this shows differently. For example, short-term memory can be broken up into different units such as visual information and acoustic information. Patient KF proves this. Patient KF was brain damaged and had problems with his short term memory. He had problems with things such as spoken numbers, letters and words and with significant sounds (such as doorbells and cats meowing). Other parts of short term memory were unaffected, such as visual (pictures).
It also shows the sensory store as a single unit whilst we know that the sensory store is split up into several different parts such as taste, vision, and hearing.
Working memory
In 1974 Baddeley and Hitch proposed a working memory model which replaced the concept of general short term memory with specific, active components. In this model, working memory consists of three basic stores: the central executive, the phonological loop and the visuo-spatial sketchpad. In 2000 this model was expanded with the multimodal episodic buffer.The central executive essentially acts as attention. It channels information to the three component processes: the phonological loop, the visuo-spatial sketchpad, and the episodic buffer.
The phonological loop stores auditory information by silently rehearsing sounds or words in a continuous loop: the articulatory process (for example the repetition of a telephone number over and over again). Then, a short list of data is easier to remember.
The visuospatial sketchpad stores visual and spatial information. It is engaged when performing spatial tasks (such as judging distances) or visual ones (such as counting the windows on a house or imagining images).
The episodic buffer is dedicated to linking information across domains to form integrated units of visual, spatial, and verbal information and chronological ordering (e.g., the memory of a story or a movie scene). The episodic buffer is also assumed to have links to long-term memory and semantical meaning.
The working memory model explains many practical observations, such as why it is easier to do two different tasks (one verbal and one visual) than two similar tasks (e.g., two visual), and the aforementioned word-length effect. However, the concept of a central executive as noted here has been criticised as inadequate and vague.
Levels of processing
Craik and Lockhart (1972) proposed that it is the method and depth of processing that affects how an experience is stored in memory, rather than rehearsal.
Classification by information type
Anderson (1976) divides long-term memory into declarative (explicit) and procedural (implicit) memories.Declarative memory requires conscious recall, in that some conscious process must call back the information. It is sometimes called explicit memory, since it consists of information that is explicitly stored and retrieved.
Declarative memory can be further sub-divided into semantic memory, which concerns facts taken independent of context; and episodic memory, which concerns information specific to a particular context, such as a time and place. Semantic memory allows the encoding of abstract knowledge about the world, such as "Paris is the capital of France". Episodic memory, on the other hand, is used for more personal memories, such as the sensations, emotions, and personal associations of a particular place or time. Autobiographical memory - memory for particular events within one's own life - is generally viewed as either equivalent to, or a subset of, episodic memory. Visual memory is part of memory preserving some characteristics of our senses pertaining to visual experience. One is able to place in memory information that resembles objects, places, animals or people in sort of a mental image. Visual memory can result in priming and it is assumed some kind of perceptual representational system underlies this phenomenon.
In contrast, procedural memory (or implicit memory) is not based on the conscious recall of information, but on implicit learning. Procedural memory is primarily employed in learning motor skills and should be considered a subset of implicit memory. It is revealed when one does better in a given task due only to repetition - no new explicit memories have been formed, but one is unconsciously accessing aspects of those previous experiences. Procedural memory involved in motor learning depends on the cerebellum and basal ganglia.
Topographic memory is the ability to orient oneself in space, to recognize and follow an itinerary, or to recognize familiar places. Getting lost when traveling alone is an example of the failure of topographic memory. This is often reported among elderly patients who are evaluated for dementia. The disorder could be caused by multiple impairments, including difficulties with perception, orientation, and memory.
Classification by temporal direction
A further major way to distinguish different memory functions is whether the content to be remembered is in the past, retrospective memory, or whether the content is to be remembered in the future, prospective memory. Thus, retrospective memory as a category includes semantic, episodic and autobiographical memory. In contrast, prospective memory is memory for future intentions, or remembering to remember (Winograd, 1988). Prospective memory can be further broken down into event- and time-based prospective remembering. Time-based prospective memories are triggered by a time-cue, such as going to the doctor (action) at 4pm (cue). Event-based prospective memories are intentions triggered by cues, such as remembering to post a letter (action) after seeing a mailbox (cue). Cues do not need to be related to the action (as the mailbox example is), and lists, sticky-notes, knotted handkerchiefs, or string around the finger are all examples of cues that are produced by people as a strategy to enhance prospective memory.
Physiology
Brain areas involved in the neuroanatomy of memory such as the hippocampus, the amygdala, the striatum, or the mammillary bodies are thought to be involved in specific types of memory. For example, the hippocampus is believed to be involved in spatial learning and declarative learning, while the amygdala is thought to be involved in emotional memory. Damage to certain areas in patients and animal models and subsequent memory deficits is a primary source of information. However, rather than implicating a specific area, it could be that damage to adjacent areas, or to a pathway traveling through the area is actually responsible for the observed deficit. Further, it is not sufficient to describe memory, and its counterpart, learning, as solely dependent on specific brain regions. Learning and memory are attributed to changes in neuronal synapses, thought to be mediated by long-term potentiation and long-term depression.Hebb distinguished between short-term and long-term memory. He postulated that any memory that stayed in short-term storage for a long enough time would be consolidated into a long-term memory. Later research showed this to be false. Research has shown that direct injections of cortisol or epinephrine help the storage of recent experiences. This is also true for stimulation of the amygdala. This proves that excitement enhances memory by the stimulation of hormones that affect the amygdala. Excessive or prolonged stress (with prolonged cortisol) may hurt memory storage. Patients with amygdalar damage are no more likely to remember emotionally charged words than nonemotionally charged ones. The hippocampus is important for explicit memory. The hippocampus is also important for memory consolidation. The hippocampus receives input from different parts of the cortex and sends its output out to different parts of the brain also. The input comes from secondary and tertiary sensory areas that have processed the information a lot already. Hippocampal damage may also cause memory loss and problems with memory storage.
Genetics
Study of the genetics of human memory is in its infancy. A notable initial success was the association of APOE with memory dysfunction in Alzheimer's Disease. The search for genes associated with normally-varying memory continues. One of the first candidates for normal variation in memory is the gene KIBRA, which appears to be associated with the rate at which material is forgotten over a delay period.
Disorders
Much of the current knowledge of memory has come from studying memory disorders. Loss of memory is known as amnesia. There are many sorts of amnesia, and by studying their different forms, it has become possible to observe apparent defects in individual sub-systems of the brain's memory systems, and thus hypothesize their function in the normally working brain. Other neurological disorders such as Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease can also affect memory and cognition. Hyperthymesia, or hyperthymesic syndrome, is a disorder which affects an individual's autobiographical memory, essentially meaning that they cannot forget small details that otherwise would not be stored. Korsakoff's syndrome, also known as Korsakoff's psychosis, amnesic-confabulatory syndrome, is an organic brain disease that adversely affects memory.While not a disorder, a common temporary failure of word retrieval from memory is the tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon. Sufferers of Nominal Aphasia (also called Anomia), however, do experience the tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon on an ongoing basis due to damage to the frontal and parietal lobes of the brain.
Methods
;Methods to optimize memorization Memorization is a method of learning that allows an individual to recall information verbatim. Rote learning is the method most often used. Methods of memorizing things have been the subject of much discussion over the years with some writers, such as Cosmos Rossellius using visual alphabets. The spacing effect shows that an individual is more likely to remember a list of items when rehearsal is spaced over an extended period of time. In contrast to this is cramming which is intensive memorisation in a short period of time. Also relevant is the Zeigarnik effect which states that people remember uncompleted or interrupted tasks better than completed ones. The so-called Method of loci uses spatial memory to memorize non-spatial information.;Interference from previous knowledge At the Center for Cognitive Science at Ohio State University, researchers have found that memory accuracy of adults is hurt by the fact that they know more than children and tend to apply this knowledge when learning new information. The findings appeared in the August 2004 edition of the journal Psychological Science.
Interference can hamper memorisation and retrieval. There is retroactive interference when learning new information causes forgetting of old information, and proactive interference where learning one piece of information makes it harder to learn similar new information.
;Influence of odors and emotions In March 2007 German researchers found they could use odors to re-activate new memories in the brains of people while they slept and the volunteers remembered better later. Emotion can have a powerful impact on memory. Numerous studies have shown that the most vivid autobiographical memories tend to be of emotional events, which are likely to be recalled more often and with more clarity and detail than neutral events.
Memory and aging
One of the key concerns of older adults is the experience of memory loss, especially as it is one of the hallmark symptoms of Alzheimer's disease. However, memory loss is qualitatively different in normal aging from the kind of memory loss associated with a diagnosis of Alzheimer's (Budson & Price, 2005).
Improving memory
A UCLA research study published in the June 2006 issue of the American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry found that people can improve cognitive function and brain efficiency through simple lifestyle changes such as incorporating memory exercises, healthy eating, physical fitness and stress reduction into their daily lives. This study examined 17 subjects, (average age 53) with normal memory performance. Eight subjects were asked to follow a "brain healthy" diet, relaxation, physical, and mental exercise (brain teasers and verbal memory training techniques). After 14 days, they showed greater word fluency (not memory) compared to their baseline performance. No long term follow up was conducted, it is therefore unclear if this intervention has lasting effects on memory.There are a loosely associated group of mnemonic principles and techniques that can be used to vastly improve memory known as the Art of memory.
The International Longevity Center released in 2001 a report which includes in pages 14–16 recommendations for keeping the mind in good functionality until advanced age. Some of the recommendations are to stay intellectually active through learning, training or reading, to keep physically active so to promote blood circulation to the brain, to socialize, to reduce stress, to keep sleep time regular, to avoid depression or emotional instability and to observe good nutrition.
Memory tasks
Paired associate learning - when one learns to associate one specific word with another. For example when given a word such as "safe" one must learn to say another specific word, such as "green". This is stimulus and response. Free recall - during this task a subject would be asked to study a list of words and then sometime later they will be asked to recall or write down as many words that they can remember. Recognition - subjects are asked to remember a list of words or pictures, after which point they are asked to identify the previously presented words or pictures from among a list of alternatives that were not presented in the original list.
See also
Footnotes
References
External links
Category:Neuropsychological assessment Category:Sources of knowledge Category:Mental processes
ar:ذاكرة (بيولوجيا) gn:Mandu'a ay:Amuyu bn:স্মৃতি be:Памяць be-x-old:Памяць bs:Memorija (biologija) bg:Памет ca:Memòria (psicologia) cs:Paměť (psychologie) da:Hukommelse de:Gedächtnis et:Mälu es:Memoria (proceso) eo:Memoro fr:Mémoire (sciences humaines) gl:Memoria ko:기억 hr:Pamćenje id:Ingatan is:Minni it:Memoria (psicologia) he:זיכרון kn:ನೆನಪು ka:მეხსიერება ku:Bîrkan la:Memoria lt:Atmintis hu:Emlékezet nl:Geheugen (psychologie) ja:記憶 no:Hukommelse pl:Pamięć człowieka pt:Memória ro:Memorie qu:Yuyay ru:Память sq:Kujtesa scn:Ricordiu simple:Memory sk:Pamäť (psychológia) sl:Spomin sr:Памћење sh:Pamćenje fi:Muisti sv:Minne ta:நினைவாற்றல் tr:İnsan belleği uk:Пам'ять yi:זכרון zh:記憶This text is licensed under the Creative Commons CC-BY-SA License. This text was originally published on Wikipedia and was developed by the Wikipedia community.